Top-Rated Free Essay
Preview

academic performance

Powerful Essays
9196 Words
Grammar
Grammar
Plagiarism
Plagiarism
Writing
Writing
Score
Score
academic performance
ARTICLE IN PRESS
Sleep Medicine Reviews (2006) 10, 323–337

www.elsevier.com/locate/smrv

CLINICAL REVIEW

Sleep loss, learning capacity and academic performance Giuseppe Curcioa,Ã, Michele Ferraraa,b, Luigi De Gennaroa a Department of Psychology, University of Rome ‘La Sapienza’, Rome, Italy
Department of Internal Medicine and Public Health, University of L’Aquila, Rome, Italy

b

KEYWORDS
Sleep deprivation;
Learning;
Memory;
Academic performance;
Neurocognitive functioning;
Adolescents;
Prefrontal cortex

Summary At a time when several studies have highlighted the relationship between sleep, learning and memory processes, an in-depth analysis of the effects of sleep deprivation on student learning ability and academic performance would appear to be essential. Most studies have been naturalistic correlative investigations, where sleep schedules were correlated with school and academic achievement. Nonetheless, some authors were able to actively manipulate sleep in order to observe neurocognitive and behavioral consequences, such as learning, memory capacity and school performance. The findings strongly suggest that: (a) students of different education levels (from school to university) are chronically sleep deprived or suffer from poor sleep quality and consequent daytime sleepiness; (b) sleep quality and quantity are closely related to student learning capacity and academic performance; (c) sleep loss is frequently associated with poor declarative and procedural learning in students; (d) studies in which sleep was actively restricted or optimized showed, respectively, a worsening and an improvement in neurocognitive and academic performance. These results may been related to the specific involvement of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) in vulnerability to sleep loss. Most methodological limitations are discussed and some future research goals are suggested. & 2006 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Introduction
Sleep is an active, repetitive and reversible behaviour serving several different functions, such
ÃCorresponding author: Laboratorio di Psicofisiologia del

Sonno, Dipartimento di Psicologia, Universita degli Studi di
`
Roma ‘La Sapienza’, Via dei Marsi 78, 00185 Roma, Italy. Tel.:
+39 06 49917508; fax: +39 06 4451667.
E-mail address: giuseppe.curcio@uniroma1.it (G. Curcio).

as repair and growth, learning or memory consolidation, and restorative processes: all these occur throughout the brain and the body.1,2 Thus, during sleep behavioural, physiological and neurocognitive processes occur: these very processes are susceptible to be impaired by the absence of sleep.
Sleep loss is, in fact, one of the most striking problems of modern society.3 Very often, to cope with our many daily interests, we prefer to sacrifice some sleep time, in the hope that this will not

1087-0792/$ - see front matter & 2006 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2005.11.001 ARTICLE IN PRESS
324
induce dangerous effects but will enable us to carry out several other activities. Unfortunately, this is not true and sleep deprivation has various consequences, such as sleepiness and impairments in neurocognitive and psychomotor performance.4,5
More specifically, in their classic meta-analysis,
Pilcher and Huffcut4 claimed that sleep-deprived individuals functioned at a level that is comparable with the ninth percentile of non-sleep-deprived subjects. These decrements in neurobehavioural functioning after sleep restriction or deprivation are well known and common to all people6,7 even though some individual differences in vulnerability to sleep loss have been shown.8
The last few years have seen an increasing literature on the relationship between sleep, memory and learning capacity (e.g. Ref.9). Recent findings have shown that sleep plays an important role in learning processes and memory consolidation,10,11 although no direct relationships were found between different kinds of memory, such as procedural or declarative memory, and different sleep stages, such as REM or NREM sleep.12 These studies clearly show that sleep deprivation can impair learning and memory for both motor procedural (e.g. Ref.10) and declarative memory systems (e.g. Ref.13).
It is well known that the integrity of learning and memory processes are fundamental in school achievement and academic performance, particularly in individuals like children and adolescents who are in a particular developmental phase.14–18
At this stage in life, adolescents suffer from increasing school, family and social pressure and from an environmentally induced delay of sleep timing,19–21 together with changes of intrinsic regulatory (both circadian and homeostatic) processes.22 Taken together, these altered sleep patterns lead to a marked increase in sleepiness23 that usually facilitates cognitive, emotional, behavioural and academic failure.22 Surprisingly, not much data exists regarding the specific effects of inadequate sleep and sleepiness on daytime functioning in children.17 Nevertheless, some experimental evidence reinforces the common belief that disrupted or poor sleep is usually followed by inefficient daytime behaviour and variability in performance. Within these activities, academic performance and/or school achievement should be carefully taken into consideration. As recently pointed out in some literature reviews,24–26 learning abilities and consequent academic performance are particularly dependent on sleep patterns and sleepiness levels. These impairments in neurocognitive functioning can be observed in several kinds of learners: from school to university students,

G. Curcio et al. so that the consequent ability to learn from lessons or from practical activities, such as laboratory work, very often proves to be drastically reduced.
The aim of this review is to focus on the effects of sleep deprivation/fragmentation on ‘day-after’ learning capacity and academic performance. In order to discuss these issues, we will briefly introduce the most recent findings on the relationships between sleep and learning-memory processes. Then, we will turn our attention to the studies showing the relationship between sleep patterns and schedules on academic performance and learning. Finally, we will discuss studies investigating the effects of induced sleep loss on academic performance and neurobehavioural functioning, with particular attention to learning capacity. The relationship between sleep and learning-memory processes
For almost a century, several studies showed the beneficial effects of sleep on memory functioning in animals and humans for different types of learning materials.9,27,28 Recent studies in molecular genetics, neurophysiology, and cognitive and behavioural neuroscience have strengthened the idea that sleep may play an important role in learning and memory, although the extent of this role remains hotly debated.29 In fact, there is still poor understanding as regards which aspects of memory function are affected by sleep and which processes underlie memory consolidation. Moreover, it is not clear whether memory consolidation is linked to a particular sleep stage, and whether different types of memory (e.g. declarative, procedural) are differentially influenced by sleep stages. Now human memory is divided into at least two branches: declarative and procedural memory.
Procedural knowledge comprises memories of how to perform some skill or how to solve a problem
(‘knowing how’). These memories, which may pertain to the motor, visual or even verbal domain,30 are usually unconsciously learned31 and are often referred to as ‘non-declarative’. Declarative material refers to memories accessible to conscious recollection (‘knowing that’). This distinction led toward the dual process hypothesis:9 the effect of sleep state on memory process would be task-dependent, with the procedural memory gaining from REM sleep10 and declarative memory linked to NREM sleep.32

ARTICLE IN PRESS
Sleep loss, learning capacity and academic performance
Since the introduction, in the last decade, of this declarative-procedural distinction in human sleep–learning investigations, there has been renewed interest in the relationship between sleep and memory/synaptic plasticity, as shown by a series of studies, mainly focusing on procedural memory testing.30,33 Firstly, it was shown that selective REM deprivation, but not SWS deprivation, abolishes the overnight performance improvement in a visual perceptual learning task, indicating that the mechanism of procedural memory consolidation depends on REM sleep.34 Interestingly, no performance improvement was observed after one night of sleep deprivation followed by two recovery nights, suggesting that sleep during the first posttraining night is mandatory for memory trace formation in this task.35 However, another study comparing retention rates following undisturbed periods of early nocturnal sleep (rich in SWS) and late nocturnal sleep (rich in REM sleep), found that only early sleep facilitated visual discrimination memory (i.e., partial sleep deprivation of late REMrich sleep did not affect performance).36 In addition, a full night of sleep (i.e., early+late sleep) further tripled this effect. These data would be in line with the alternative point of view, i.e. the hypothesis of a sequential processing of memories during sleep stages,37,38 suggesting that memory formation is prompted by SWS and then consolidated by REM sleep. Accordingly, the amount of sleep-dependent improvement on the perceptual learning task is linearly correlated with the amount of SWS during the first quarter of the night, and with the amount of REM sleep in the last quarter.39
Also the NREM-REM sleep sequence seems to be important for the retention of declarative material, since morning recall of pairs of unrelated words is impaired only when sleep fragmentation leads to sleep cycle disorganization, but not when awakenings during the night preserved the sleep cycles.40
The beneficial effect of sleep on procedural memory has been investigated also by means of motor skill learning tasks. A finger-tapping task41 or a nearly identical finger-to-thumb opposition task have been used.42 A sleep-dependent improvement in performance 24 h after training without further practice has been demonstrated; it is seen only after nocturnal sleep but not across nocturnal wakefulness.42,43 Interestingly, performance improvement was proportional to the time spent in
REM sleep. Other researchers reported that an overnight improvement on FTT was correlated with the amount of stage 2 NREM during the whole night, and especially during the last quarter of the night.41 Finally, it has been reported that performance in other tasks of verbal procedural memory

325

(word stem priming) and visuo-motor procedural memory (recall of mirror tracing skills) is improved most after late REM-rich sleep, compared with early sleep or with equivalent wake retention intervals.32,44 On the other hand, in an elegant study, Huber and co-workers45 gave subjects—prior to sleep—a complex procedural motor adaptation task requiring hand-eye coordination. During posttraining sleep, only slow-wave activity (SWA) increased in the right parietal lobe, i.e. the involved area for this kind of task. The extent of the local parietal increase in slow-wave activity in the first 90 min of sleep also strongly correlated with the subsequent amount of performance enhancement (learning) observed the next day, showing a close relationship between local EEG activity and subsequent regional SWA homeostasis.
The effects of sleep on memory for declarative material have received much less attention in recent years, although many of the earlier experiments often investigated the effects of classic tests of declarative memory (e.g. verbal learning tasks) on REM sleep changes following training.10 Their findings are mixed and quite contradictory. Even when the effects of REM sleep deprivation and
NREM sleep interruption were compared,28,46 no differential effects of the two sleep manipulations on declarative memory were reported. More recently, verbal (recall of paired-associate word lists) and non-verbal declarative memory (recall of spatial locations in a mental rotation task) were investigated and showed more improvement across an interval covering early sleep than across late sleep and corresponding intervals of wakefulness.32,44 These results were interpreted as indicating that, more than REM sleep, SWS exerts a selective facilitation of declarative memory consolidation in humans. However, it may be that not
SWS per se, but the inhibition of glucocorticoid release from the adrenals that characterizes early nocturnal sleep, is responsible for memory consolidation. In fact, elevating plasma glucocorticoid concentration during early sleep by administering cortisol47 or dexamethasone48 blocked the beneficial effect of early sleep on the recall of paired associates. On the whole, these results have generally been interpreted as supporting a fundamental role of
REM for the consolidation of procedural memories in humans, while SWS-rich sleep seems to have facilitating effects for declarative memories.30,33
However, several contradictions are still to be resolved. The first regards the actual role of REM sleep for procedural memory consolidation. As a matter of fact, visual discrimination task improvements have been related both to REM sleep34 and

ARTICLE IN PRESS
326
SWS,36 while others suggested that both stages are needed to obtain the maximal benefits.36,39 Moreover, motor skill performance improvements have been related either to REM sleep42 and stage 2.41,49
Even the beneficial effects of late sleep on visuomotor and verbal procedural tasks, interpreted as supporting a role for REM sleep in the consolidation of these types of memory,32,44 can also be viewed as reflecting the involvement of stage 2, as the time spent in both stages is nearly equivalent during the second half of the night. Consequently, in our view, the idea that REM sleep is the most important for procedural memory consolidation30,33 remains speculative and should be further investigated.
Similarly, evidence for the involvement of SWS in the consolidation of verbal and non-verbal declarative memory32,44 is far from definitive since, at the present time, it cannot be ruled out that residual amounts of stage 2 or REM sleep during early sleep may contribute to the observed effects.
So, it may be concluded that both REM and NREM sleep are necessary for learning and memory, as some authors have clearly pointed out,35 and that for an efficient consolidation of both (declarative) knowledge and (procedural) skills, the worst enemy is sleep loss or, also, sleep fragmentation.

Sleep patterns/schedules and academic performance As seen in the previous section, sleep has a relevant facilitating role in learning and memory processes.
Conversely, sleep deprivation and/or fragmentation usually impairs these functions. In the following, we will review the most relevant contributions in the literature investigating the effects of sleep patterns and schedules on academic performance of school and university students. It should be stressed that most of these studies correlated sleep-wake patterns with subjective (self- or parent-reported) academic achievement or with rough estimates of behaviours associated with daytime sleepiness, and, as a consequence, they are intrinsically correlational.

Academic performance from school to university School achievement has been measured by using different parameters: grade point average (GPA), self-reported average grades, teacher comments/ behaviour ratings, parent reports, and school behaviour.26 Unfortunately, the use of such a

G. Curcio et al. plethora of different measures is a limitation to the comparability between different studies. Moreover, rating systems vary across different schools and, consequently, even GPAs appear to be nonobjective indices. Thus, the only way to strengthen the findings would be by using multiple measures in the same study.26
Based on the responses to a questionnaire filled in by the parents of students, Kahn and co-workers50 compared health, behaviour, sleep complaints and school performance of normal and poor (longer sleep latencies and frequent nocturnal awakenings) sleepers. A percentage of 21% of poor sleepers failed 1 or more years at school, while similar problems were observed in just 11% of normal sleepers. Moreover, school achievement difficulties resulted more frequent in poor than in normal sleepers. One of the best predictors for this low school achievement has been identified in children’s fatigue, i.e. difficulties in morning arousal and the need for afternoon naps.51
As an indirect link between sleep and academic performance, it was shown that students with more regular sleep-wake patterns (shorter sleep latencies, fewer night awakenings, later school rise times, earlier rise times on weekends) reported higher GPA,52,53 whereas students with lower grades reported increased daytime sleepiness, also as a consequence of shorter sleeping nights.52
An important contribution was offered by Wolfson and Carskadon,15 who studied (by means of a health and behaviour survey with self-reported grades) sleep patterns and daytime functioning in about 3000 high school students. They showed that students with higher grades reported more total sleep, earlier bedtimes on school nights and reduced weekend delays of sleep schedules than students with lower grades. These data of better performance as a consequence of ‘hygienic’ sleep patterns were confirmed by several studies indicating a poorer school achievement, a greater tendency to fall asleep in school, more difficulties in concentration and in focusing attention in students with an evening circadian typology,54 earlier school starting times55,56 and a mild delayed sleep phase syndrome.57 Another recent study using surveys and selfratings58 pointed out that time in bed has no relationship with facility of concentration or paying attention at school, whereas sleep quality and feeling rested at school (index of a good night’s sleep) were highly related to a general measure of school functioning. Moreover, children without difficulty getting up displayed more achievement motivation. This was one of the first studies that explicitly stressed the relevance of sleep quality

ARTICLE IN PRESS
Sleep loss, learning capacity and academic performance and continuity on school functioning, giving relatively less importance to the merely quantitative amount of sleep (as observed also by Epstein et al.56 and Pilcher et al.59).
In a study with both interviews and surveys administered to first-year college students,60 researchers observed that sleep habits were highly correlated with academic performance. The official grades (provided by the university register) showed that students with a lower performance were those with later bedtimes and wake-up times on both weekdays and weekend days. The authors proposed that, for each hour of delay in reported rise time during the week, the predicted GPAs could decrease by 0.13 on a scale of 0–4, hypothesizing that this delay could induce a diminished ability to recall complex material learned earlier in class, as suggested by Dotto.61
The first study using an objective measure for evaluating sleep patterns of school students was by
Sadeh and colleagues.62 They actigraphically monitored 140 children for 4–5 consecutive nights and collected questionnaires and daily reports filled in by both children and their parents. The main finding was that older children had more delayed sleep onset times and increased daytime sleepiness. These effects were mainly explained on the basis of a conspicuous incidence (18%) of sleep fragmentation. On the basis of these findings, the authors concluded that a strong association does exist between altered child sleep patterns and daytime sleepiness, suggesting a possible causal link between sleep loss and learning/attentional deficits. In a sample of 5813 healthy Finnish children,
Paavonen and collegues63 identified 17.8% of selfreported sleep complaints (mainly problems of sleep onset, enuresis, night awakenings). These self-evaluated problems correlated significantly with teachers’ reports about a reduced academic performance in students with severe sleep complaints as compared to normal sleepers. Similar data were observed in another study,64 where it was seen that short sleepers (p6 h per night) obtained lower grades (GPA: 2.74) than long sleepers (X9 h per night; GPA:3.24), indicating that those who have more total night sleep tend to have higher grades.
On the other hand, others reported no correlation between total sleep time and GPAs.65 The authors administered a one-page questionnaire on sleep characteristics and on self-reported school achievement to 1200 high and middle school students; their findings did not support any association between sleep amount and academic performance. 327

A recent epidemiological survey on Italian highschool students,66 showed that adolescents reporting a reduced academic performance mainly explained by attention problems in the classroom, tend to have more irregular bedtimes and, consequently, sleep significantly less than their peers who did not complain about attention problems.
These data confirm results obtained by the same group in preliminary studies,67,68 where school achievement and daytime functioning were related to irregular sleep schedules, shorter sleep times, later bedtimes and increased daytime sleepiness, in both children and adolescents.
Steenari and collegues69 investigated the link between auditory and visual working memory and sleep in a Finnish population of 6 to 13 year-old students. As pointed out by the authors, working memory is necessary in several aspects of daily behaviour, such as learning, reasoning, language comprehension and acquisition of reading ability.
Based on actigraphic data, the authors showed that lower sleep efficiency and longer sleep latency were associated with a higher percentage of incorrect responses at all load levels of the task, whereas shorter sleep duration affected only performance at the more-demanding level of the task. Although most correlations were not statistically robust, they were strong enough to be considered of real practical importance.
An interesting study by Sadeh and collegues70 investigated the associations between sleep and neurobehavioural functioning (NBF) in 135 adolescents aged between 7.2 and 12.7 years. They used an objective, although indirect, measure of sleep
(actigraphy) and correlated it with NBF by comparing three groups of different age. Neurobehavioural evaluation was based on several different tasks
(motor speed, sustained attention, concentration, memory and learning span, etc) and was compared between good and poor sleepers62 and within each considered age group. Results did not show any association between sleep duration and/or sleep schedule and NBF as observed by other previous studies.15,56,71,72 Conversely, a significant correlation was seen between measures of sleep quality and sleep fragmentation with performance decrease reflecting attentional deficit and compromised executive control.17,73 These effects were much more evident in younger students, who resulted more vulnerable to insufficient sleep.
Another recent study, conducted on 3871 highschool students in Seoul, showed a strong presence of poor sleep quantity and quality, with a consequent sharp increase in daytime sleepiness, that correlates significantly with a decline in academic performance.74 A similar relation between

ARTICLE IN PRESS
328
increased self-reported daytime sleepiness and low grades was seen in a validation study for a new sleepiness scale (Pediatric Daytime Sleepiness
Scale, PDSS) conducted on 450 middle school students.75 Moreover, by using GPAs on an introductory course of psychology and sleep self-ratings, Howell and co-workers76 confirmed the correlation between poor sleep quality and reduced academic measures. Similar findings were observed in a
Spanish study, where students with sleep complaints, rating themselves tired upon awakening and with daytime sleepiness, tended to fail in class with respect to those who reported a good night’s sleep.77 All these studies showed that increasing daytime sleepiness, as a consequence of poor quality of sleep, can seriously impair students’ cognitive functioning and behavioural performance (e.g.
Ref.15). Academic performance is, in fact, clearly linked to sleep habits and daytime sleepiness levels.14,17 This relationship is indirectly supported also by several findings provided by the study of children with sleep-breathing disorders.
Sleep-disordered breathing (SDB) is a spectrum of disorders ranging from primary snoring to severe obstructive sleep apnoea syndrome (OSAS): 10–25% of 3–12 year-old children suffer from primary snoring and 10% of these children can have OSAS.78
SDB is known to be associated with several behavioural problems, reduced academic achievement79 and neurocognitive impairments such as learning, memory and problem-solving.80,81 As an example, Urschitz and collegues82 showed that snoring and intermittent hypoxia in children were significantly associated with school failures. Specifically, snorers had twice the risk of performing poorly at school: this association became stronger with increasing snoring frequency.82 Differences in acquisition or recalling new information, rather than difficulties in attention, were hypothesized to be the consequence of sleep fragmentation and repeated night-time arousals, that may adversely impact learning and memory tasks.82–84 In another study, a group of 54 children with SDB was investigated with respect to academic performance.9 Among them, only 24 patients were treated with an adenotonsillectomy. At the follow-up, 1 year later, academic performance and grades resulted significantly improved only in the treated group. This study shows that children with academic problems due to night-time breathing disorders may benefit from prospective medical treatment.79 Similar results of substantial cognitive improvement after adenotonsillectomy were observed in children with OSAS.85 Unfortunately,

G. Curcio et al. these effects of SDB-associated neurocognitive morbidity may only be partially reversible: a learning deficit can develop during early childhood and hinder subsequent school achievement.86
Similar negative effects on learning and academic performance were proposed for both obesity87 and allergic rhinitis.88

Effects of imposed sleep loss on learning and academic performance
Results from studies investigating the effects of different sleep patterns and schedules on academic performance showed that students who sleep poorly, with an elevated sleep fragmentation
(i.e., reduced sleep quality), with later bedtimes and early awakenings, usually tend to offer a decreased academic performance and a reduced neurobehavioural functioning.14,25,26 However, the studies seen so far are only ‘naturalistic’ correlative ones: in this section we shall more deeply review those studies which have tried to experimentally manipulate sleep amount in order to evaluate the effects on subsequent cognitive functioning. These studies are summarized in
Table 1.
The oldest study investigating the effects of total sleep deprivation (50 h) in students’ motor performance89 showed significant impairments in psychomotor abilities: such a decrease appeared after only 18 h of wakefulness on reaction times. Other measures required a longer deprivation to show detrimental effects: endurance (34 h of continuous wake), agility, balance and power (42 h), speed
(after 50 h).
Nevertheless, the first study which examined the effect of sleep loss on adolescents’ psychomotor and cognitive performances was carried out by
Carskadon and co-workers.90 The authors assessed the sleepiness (through the Multiple Sleep Latency
Test, MSLT, and subjective ratings) and performance (problem-solving/computational ability, memory, auditory attention, sustained motor activity) of 12 adolescents during 38 h of total sleep deprivation. Results showed an increase in both objective and subjective sleepiness, whereas performance was affected only on memory and computational speed. No statistically significant changes were observed on attention and sustained motor activity. The authors concluded that the response of adolescents to relatively mild sleep deprivation is similar to that of older subjects: as a possible explanation for these data, the authors proposed the so-called ‘lapse hypothesis’,91 i.e.

ARTICLE IN PRESS
Sleep loss, learning capacity and academic performance the presence of brief lapses in attention during task completion, that could greatly slow down the subjects’ answers. Moreover, similar impairments in problem-solving have recently been observed in adults,92,93 for which an involvement of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) has been proposed.17
The same group94 also investigated the effect of an acute restriction (one night of 4 h sleep). Nine children were submitted to the same task battery as in the previous study,90 but they did not show any significant effect after this sleep restriction schedule. Polysomnography indicated that total sleep time increased as a consequence of sleep manipulation, while sleep latency and night-time awakenings were reduced. The authors claimed that these findings suggest that sleep restriction may not always lead to observable decrements as, instead, total sleep deprivation does.
After a total sleep deprivation of one night, 44 college students showed the expected significant decrease of performance on cognitive tasks assessing inference, recognition of assumptions and deduction.95 Interestingly, although the sleep-deprived subjects performed worse, they reported higher levels of estimated performance and more effort expended on tasks than the non-deprived subjects. This study raises the interesting issue of a reduced awareness of the extent to which sleep loss can impair the ability to complete cognitive tasks. Such a phenomenon suggests that many students could damage their own academic performance by choosing the wrong schedule to deprive themselves before examinations or class work.95
The first study aimed at directly assessing learning capacity after sleep loss was done by
Randazzo and co-workers.96 Sixteen children were asked to sleep only 5 h for a single night and then their psychomotor and cognitive performance was assessed, together with sleep propensity as evaluated by the MSLT. Tasks measured attention, vigilance, abstract thinking, memory, learning and creativity. As a consequence of sleep restriction, subjects showed shorter sleep latencies and an impaired performance on verbal creativity (mainly fluency and flexibility) and on abstract thinking.
Although easier psychomotor tasks as well as memory and learning tests failed to show differences after sleep curtailment, it should be pointed out that scores in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test
(WCST) were significantly affected, indicating a difficulty in learning new abstract concepts. The authors concluded by observing that a moderate sleep restriction can affect the children’s executive functions that ‘yenable the individual to engage in creative, adaptive learning by initiating and regulating retrieval of knowledge from long-term

329

memory, modifying the knowledge base, and mediating problem-solving’.96 Surprisingly, these effects were clear, despite a strong motivational effort suggested by a good performance on lowdemanding tasks. Since verbal fluency was affected, then here too the key cerebral region could be the PFC.
A subsequent work by the same authors97 published as an abstract, investigated the effects on cognitive functioning after 3 nights of mild sleep restriction (7 h) on children aged 10–14 years.
Results confirmed impairments in verbal fluency and creativity, while no effects were found in a working memory task, computational accuracy and planning ability. Thus, also in an extended schedule of sleep restriction, only higher cognitive functions seem sensitive to sleep loss.
Fallone and co-workers98 evaluated the effects of an acute (one-night) sleep restriction on sustained attention, response inhibition and on a simulated academic exercise in children aged 8–15 years. They compared two groups of subjects with an Optimized (10 h) and a Restricted (4 h) sleep schedule. Children in the Restricted group resulted more inattentive and showed a higher frequency of observed sleepy behaviours during a simulated academic situation. Moreover, the MSLTs showed an increased sleep propensity in the Restricted group; an increase in sleepiness was also observed in subjective ratings. The authors concluded that sleepiness following acute sleep restriction is not sufficient to produce deficits on performance measures of impulsivity and sustained attention and that these data do not support the hypothesized prefrontal cortical impairment after sleep loss.17 The same group performed a study with a prolonged sleep restriction.99 In this case 27 children were assigned to an Optimized (10 h per night) or Restricted Group (6.5 h per night) and they were asked to maintain their sleep schedule for 6 consecutive nights. Their behaviour was assessed by parents, teachers and in-lab staff, while measures of sleepiness were based on MSLTs and subjective ratings. Results showed that restricting sleep each night to 6.5 h for one week was associated with daytime sleepiness, inattentiveness and academic problems. Again, the main limitation of this study is the fact that it is mainly based on parents and teachers’ reports.
After noting a close correlation between objectively assessed sleep quality and NBF in a naturalistic design, Sadeh and co-workers100 proceeded to an experimental manipulation of sleep amount, assuming that cumulative (3 nights) sleep restriction or extension might lead to, respectively,

N

15

12

9

23

8

Copes and
Rosentswieg,89

Carskadon et al.90

Carskadon et al.94

Pilcher and Walters,95

Randazzo et al.96

SR: one night (5 hs of sleep allowed)

TSD: one night (24 hs of continuous wakefulness) SR: one night (4 hs sleep alowed)

TSD: one night (38 hs of continuous wakefulness) TSD: 2 nights (50 hs of continuous wakefulness)

Sleep manipulation

PSG; MSLT

n.a.

PSG; MSLT; sleepiness subjective ratings

PSG; MSLT; sleepiness subjective ratings

n.a.

Sleep/sleepiness variables Attention, vigilance, abstract thinking, memory, learning, creativity Motor performance
(accuracy, agility, balance, endurance power, RT, speed, strength) Problem-solving or computational ability, memory, auditory attention, sustained motor activity Problem-solving or computational ability, memory, auditory attention, sustained motor activity Performance at a critical thinking task

Academic/cognitive and performance variables Reduced cognitive achievement; reduced awareness of cognitive impairments
Increase in sleep propensity; impaired performance on verbal creativity and abstract thinking

Increase in sleepiness; changes on REC sleep architecture; poor memory and computational speed
Increase in sleepiness; changes on REC sleep architecture Impairments of psychomotor abilities: RT the most sensitive Key findings

330

10–14 (11.6)

n.a. (20.5)

11–13.2 (12.0)

11.7–14.6 (13.5)

n.a. (15.2)

Age range (mean)

Studies evaluating cognitive achievement after sleep manipulations.

Study

Table 1

ARTICLE IN PRESS
G. Curcio et al.

27

45

37

Fallone et al.99

Fallone et al.98

Sadeh et al.100
9.1–12.2 (n.a.)

8–15 (12.0)

11–13 (n.a.)

10–14 (n.a.)

SR: three night (1 h less than their habitual schedule)

SR: one night (4 hs of sleep allowed)

SR: six nights (6.5 hs of sleep allowed)

SR: three nights (7 hs of sleep allowed)

PSG

PSG; MSLT; sleepiness subjective ratings

MSLT; sleepiness subjective ratings

n.a.

On verbal and figural creativity, working memory task, computational accuracy; working memory, RT, vigilance, planning ability and perceptual organization
Parents, teachers and in-lab staff assessment of subjects’ behaviour
Sustained attention, response inhibition and behaviour in a simulated academic exercise Motor speed, vigilance and motor reaction, sustained visual attention, response inhibition, visual memory and visual-motor speed, working memory, learning strategies

n.a.: not available; PSG: polysomnography; REC: revovery night; RT: reaction times; SR: sleep restriction; TSD: total sleep deprivation.

20

Randazzo et al.97

Increase of daytime sleepiness, inattentiveness and academic problems
Increase in daytime sleepiness and sleep propensity; increased inattentive behaviours
Decrease in sleep quality, vigilance, attention, memory

Impaired performance on verbal fluency and creativity ARTICLE IN PRESS

Sleep loss, learning capacity and academic performance
331

ARTICLE IN PRESS
332
cumulative negative or positive effects on NBF.101
Subjects were requested to extend or restrict their habitual sleep for 1 hour: this manipulation of sleep time resulted in a mean decrease of 41 minutes for the restricted group, and a mean increase of
35 minutes for the extended group. Subjects were then tested with the same battery (Neuropsychological Evaluation System, NES) used in a previous work.70 Results showed that this modest experimental manipulation led to distinct effects on performance patterns: in the extension condition, vigilance, attention and memory resulted improved with respect to the sleep restriction condition. The authors claimed that these results have significant implications for learning and school performance, since these tasks are highly correlated with classroom behaviours and achievement tests.100
Academic performance is also dependent on school starting time and the delayed sleep phase.
As recently reviewed by Wolfson and Carskadon,26 in fact, children with an early school start time reported much more daytime sleepiness, dozing in class, attention difficulties and poorer academic performance. For example, a study using questionnaires56 compared the effects of different school starting times: results indicated that the
‘early risers’ slept significantly less (about 24 min) than ‘regular risers’. As a result, the ‘early risers’ complained more about daytime sleepiness and fatigue and about difficulties in concentrating and paying attention during classes, with a two-fold incidence of dozing off in class. Similar results were observed in the previously discussed Italian survey.66 Finally, a recent study carried out in the
US102 showed that school starting time caused students to lose up to 2 h sleep per night, but only on weekdays: during weekends, in fact, a kind of recovery occurred. Moreover, as a consequence of school starting time and of early awakening, students provided a better neurocognitive performance in the afternoon than in the morning.
Another facet of the problem is related to the phase delay which is very often seen in young people. This tendency to extend the weekend’s sleep schedules (late lights-out time, long sleeponset latency, late wake-up time) to weekdays is usually called the delayed sleep phase syndrome
(DSPS), a habit that perpetuates both sleep onset and awakening problems. An incidence of 17% of
DSPS was reported in a university sample.57 In this subgroup, a delay of over 90 min in bedtime and waking time at weekends compared to weeknights was observed. This marked delay induced an impaired academic performance (lower grades) in students with DSPS compared to a control group. A similar prevalence of DSPS in the population

G. Curcio et al.
(11.5%) was found in a U.S. study,103 whose authors suggested a strong role of college lifestyle in the development of this syndrome.
Unfortunately, no studies were carried out with the aim of comparing different sleep/wake schedules, i.e. imposed (or ‘optimal’) versus nonimposed (chosen by the children/control).
Again, some conclusions can be made on the basis of the literature review. Firstly, poor or fragmented sleep is often associated with behavioural and cognitive difficulties. Secondly, these impairments often reduce academic achievement and learning. Finally, this decrease in neurocognitive functioning can easily be reverted by adopting healthy sleep schedules (fixed bedtimes and waking times, no chronic sleep restriction, etc.).
These conclusions are also related to the daily sleep need in modern life, an issue hotly debated with regard to both the adult (e.g. Ref.104) and young population.22 It is now well known that during puberty and adolescence both SWS and REM sleep start to decrease.105 At the same time, also sleep quantity and quality result greatly impaired, as a consequence of a dramatic delay of sleep timing induced by increasing psychosocial and environmental demands on children.14 This cascades into a chronic pattern of insufficient schoolday sleep, forced arousals at a biologically inappropriate time, with negative impacts on adolescent cognition, performance, mood and behaviour.22 Conclusions
The studies discussed here allow us to draw some conclusions. As a first observation, regardless of the theoretical framework adopted (dual process hypothesis or sequential processing hypothesis), both
REM and NREM sleep seem necessary for learning and memory: thus, for an efficient consolidation of both (declarative) knowledge and (procedural) skills, the worst risk is sleep loss or fragmentation.
Moreover, it was shown that an increasing daytime sleepiness, as a consequence of poor sleep quality, can seriously impair students’ cognitive functioning and behavioural performance. An association between academic performance and sleep habits or daytime sleepiness levels has also been suggested by children’s sleep-breathing disorders or obesity.
Finally, studies with experimental manipulations of the amount and quality of children’s sleep confirmed that poor or fragmented sleep is associated with behavioural and cognitive difficulties,

ARTICLE IN PRESS
Sleep loss, learning capacity and academic performance with reduced academic achievement and learning.
Nonetheless, this decrease in neurocognitive functioning can easily be reverted by adopting healthy sleep schedules, such as fixed bedtimes and waking times, fixed school starting times, and by limiting psychosocial and environmental pressure.
Unfortunately, several methodological limits do exist in this literature. Different indicators are usually used in assessing academic performance: self-reported or actually recorded GPAs (provided by the register), self-reported average grades, teachers’ comments/behaviour ratings, parents’ reports, and school behaviour.26 This is a real problem that should be resolved in the near future, since the different outcome measures reported in the literature scarcely overlap: as an example,
Wolfson and Carskadon26 claimed that even grades, seen as the most reliable measure, are usually not equivalent in different school systems. Nevertheless, they do reflect actual child learning abilities since they are closely connected to the school curriculum.106 On the other hand, others suggest that achievement tests should be a better measure because they are individually administered (thus more reliable for children with learning difficulties) and are more representative of the material taught.107 Thus, a mandatory need for this field of research is to find a consistent measure for learning capacity and academic performance. Alternatively, in order to obtain broader and more reliable evaluations, a multi-measure approach should be adopted, with grades, tests, as well as both teacher and parent self-reports (as proposed in Ref.26).
Moreover, although a great number of studies using surveys have been conducted, very few studies (see Table 1) used an experimental manipulation of sleep timing. Thus, this field needs many more studies investigating specific schoollike tasks with laboratory-based paradigms, not neglecting the possibility of a kind of ‘Hawthorne effect’.25,26 Only in this way will it be possible to highlight the actual causal relationship between sleep features, learning capacity and academic achievement. Finally, some confounding variables should also be taken into consideration. Wolfson and Carskadon quite reasonably proposed26 the relevant role of environmental and family variables. When comparing students coming from different schools (with different evaluation systems, class size, ethnic presence), different families (with a permissive or an authoritarian style) and with different psychosocial commitments (i.e. extra-curricular activities), a serious bias can be introduced. To avoid these methodological problems, a more careful sample selection is needed.

333

In humans, several studies have demonstrated that neural systems of the prefrontal cortex involved in executive functioning are more susceptible to sleep deprivation.5,7 As a result, most neurocognitive functions result impaired by acute sleep loss, such as attention and divergent thinking,108 language,109,110 decision-making,5 memory and response inhibition,111 and serial subtraction.112 These data have largely been confirmed by neuroimaging studies (e.g. Refs.113,114). Moreover, it is well known that under chronic partial sleep deprivation, neurocognitive deficits can accumulate over time,6,94 even if a subjective adaptation to the sensation of sleepiness can be experienced. Thus, these effects could be present also in students.17 In these populations, a PFC deficit could lead to impairments in learning, attention, decision-making and complex, divergent or creative thinking. As a first consequence, academic performance will result reduced or impoverished, and a further effect may also be seen on emotional and social processing,
i.e. in the PFC highest level of integration.17
Moreover, these effects could result in a dangerous cumulative effect that is particularly significant for children, who need much more time than adults to fully recover their normal neurocognitive functioning. Practice points
1. Students of different education levels (from school to university) are chronically sleep deprived or suffer from poor sleep quality: this is also due to psychosocial, environmental and professional pressure.
2. Sleep quality and quantity induce several effects on a student’s life: increased daytime sleepiness, impaired mood, neurocognitive deficits and behavioural changes.
3. The effects of sleep loss are mainly evident on higher cognitive functions (attention, memory, problem-solving, etc); as a result, learning capacity and academic performance may seriously be affected.
4. Studies in which sleep was actively restricted or optimized showed, respectively, a worsening and an improvement in neurocognitive and academic performance: this raises the possibility of an improvement in neurocognitive functioning (and thus of learning capacity and academic performance) as a consequence of healthy sleep schedules. ARTICLE IN PRESS
334
Research agenda
1. As a first step, it is necessary to find reliable measures of learning capacity and academic performance. Alternatively, a multimeasure approach (grades, tests, teacher and parent self-reports) could be adopted.
2. Moreover, more studies are needed investigating specific school-like tasks with laboratory-based paradigms. This is important in order to pinpoint the actual causal relationship between sleep fragmentation, sleep restriction and learning capacity and academic achievement.
3. Not alternatively, investigations should be carried out on patients with sleep disorders and/or other sleep fragmentation problems, such as sleep apnea or sleep-breathing disorders, restless legs syndrome, obesity, etc.
4. Also, it could be very interesting to carry out prospective studies comparing different imposed sleep/wake schedules (regular or
‘optimal’) to non- imposed schedules (such as a control) over a complete semester/ year, to investigate their differential effects on academic achievement.
5. Finally, an important goal for future research is the study of the so-called longterm effects. Clear answers to the following questions are needed: ‘What does chronic sleep deprivation induce in students’ learning capacity?’, ‘Are there such things as developmental time-windows that are particularly sensitive to sleep disruption and, consequently, critical for adult academic performance?’, ‘Does sleep fragmentation in the critical developmental period impair long-term efficiency of PFC-related functions?’. Acknowledgements
We wish to thank Giancarlo La Camera, Ph.D. (NIH,
Bethesda, USA) and Caterina Cinel, Ph.D. (University of Essex, UK) for their invaluable help in collecting some of the bibliographic material.

References
1. Krueger JM, Obal F. Sleep function. Front Biosci
2003;8:d511–9.
2. Benington JH. Sleep homeostasis and the function of sleep. Sleep 2000;23:959–66.

G. Curcio et al.
3. Bonnet MH. Sleep Deprivation. In: Kryger MH, Roth T,
Dement WC, editors. Principles and Practice of Sleep
Medicine. 3rd ed. London: Saunders; 2000. p. 53–71.
4. Pilcher JJ, Huffcut AI. Effects of sleep deprivation on performance: a meta-analysis. Sleep 1996;19:318–26.
5. Harrison Y, Horne JA. The impact of sleep deprivation on decision making: a review. J Exp Psychol Appl 2000;6:
236–49.
6. Van Dongen HP, Maislin G, Mullington JM, et al. The cumulative cost of additional wakefulness: dose-response effects on neurobehavioral functions and sleep physiology from chronic sleep restriction and total sleep deprivation.
Sleep 2003;26:117–26.
7. Durmer JS, Dinges DF. Neurocognitive consequences of sleep deprivation. Semin Neurol 2005;25:117–29.
8. Van Dongen HP, Baynard MD, Maislin G, et al. Systematic interindividual differences in neurobehavioral impairment from sleep loss: evidence of trait-like differential vulnerability. Sleep 2004;27:423–33.
9. Peigneux P, Laureys S, Delbeuck X, et al. Sleeping brain, learning brain. The role of sleep for memory systems.
Neuroreport 2001;12:A111–24.
10. Smith C. Sleep states and memory processes in humans: procedural versus declarative memory systems. Sleep Med
Rev 2001;5:491–506.
11. Hobson JA, Pace-Schott EF. The cognitive neuroscience of sleep:neuronal systems, counsciousness and learning.
Nature Rev Neurosci 2002;3:679–93.
12. Rauchs G, Desgranges B, Foret J, et al. The relationships between memory systems and sleep stages. J Sleep Res
2005;14:123–40.
13. Gais S, Born J. Declarative memory consolidation: mechanisms acting during human sleep. Learn Mem
2004;11:679–85.
14. Carskadon MA. Patterns of sleep and sleepiness in adolescents. Pediatrician 1990;17:5–12.
Ã15. Wolfson AR, Carskadon MA. Sleep schedules and daytime functioning in adolescents. Child Dev 1998;69:875–87.
16. Carskadon MA, Vieira C, Acebo C. Association between puberty and delayed phase preference. Sleep 1993;16:
258–62.
Ã17. Dahl RE. The impact of inadequate sleep on children’s daytime cognitive function. Semin Pediatr Neurol 1996;
3:44–50.
18. Dahl RE. The development and disorders of sleep. Adv
Pediatr 1998;45:73–90.
19. Andrade MM, Benedito-Silva AA, Domenice S, et al. Sleep characteristics of adolescents: a longitudinal study.
J Adolesc Health 1993;14:401–6.
20. Dahl RE, Lewin DS. Pathways to adolescent health sleep regulation and behavior. J Adolesc Health 2002;31(Suppl):
175–84.
21. Gau SF, Soong WT. The transition of sleep-wake patterns in early adolescence. Sleep 2003;26:449–54.
Ã22. Carskadon MA, Acebo C, Jenni OG. Regulation of adolescent sleep: implications for behavior. Ann N Y Acad
Sci 2004;1021:276–91.
Ã23. Carskadon MA, Acebo C. Regulation of sleepiness in adolescents: update, insights, and speculation. Sleep
2002;25:606–14.
24. Samkoff JS, Jacques CH. A review of studies concerning effects of sleep deprivation and fatigue on residents’ performance. Acad Med 1991;66:687–93.
ÃThe most important references are denoted by an asterisk.

ARTICLE IN PRESS
Sleep loss, learning capacity and academic performance
25. Fallone G, Owens JA, Deane J. Sleepiness in children and adolescents: clinical implications. Sleep Med Rev 2002;6:
287–306.
Ã26. Wolfson AR, Carskadon MA. Understanding adolescents’ sleep patterns and school performance: a critical appraisal. Sleep Med Rev 2003;7:491–506.
27. Jenkins J, Dallenbach K. Oblivescence during sleep and waking. Amer J Psychol 1924;35:605–12.
28. Smith C. Sleep states and memory processes. Behav Brain
Res 1995;69:137–45.
29. Siegel J. The REM sleep-memory consolidation. Science
2001;294:1058–63.
30. Born J, Gais S. Roles of early and late nocturnal sleep for the consolidation of human memories. In: Maquet P, Smith
C, Stickgold R, editors. Sleep and brain plasticity. Oxford:
Oxford University Press; 2003. p. 65–85.
31. Squire L. Declarative and nondeclarative memory: multiple brain systems supporting learning and memory. J Cogn
Neurosci 1992;4:232–43.
32. Plihal W, Born J. Effect of early and late nocturnal sleep on declarative and procedural memory. J Cogn Neurosci
1997;9:534–47.
33. Stickgold R. Human studies of sleep and off-line memory processing. In: Maquet P, Smith C, Stickgold R, editors.
Sleep and brain plasticity. Oxford: Oxford University
Press; 2003. p. 41–63.
34. Karni A, Tanne D, Rubenstein BS, et al. Dependence on
REM sleep of overnight improvement of a perceptual skill.
Science 1994;265:679–82.
35. Stickgold R, James L, Hobson J. Visual discrimination learning requires sleep after training. Nat Neurosci
2000;3:1237–8.
36. Gais S, Plihal W, Wagner U, et al. Early sleep triggers memory for early visual discrimination skills. Nat Neurosci
2000;3:1335–9.
37. Giuditta A, Ambrosini MV, Montagnese P, et al. The sequential hypothesis of the function of sleep. Behav
Brain Res 1995;69:157–66.
38. Ambrosini MV, Giuditta A. Learning and sleep: the sequential hypothesis. Sleep Med Rev 2001;5:477–90.
39. Stickgold R, Whidbee D, Schirmer B, et al. Visual discrimination task improvement: A multi-step process occurring during sleep. J Cogn Neurosci 2000;12:
246–54.
40. Ficca G, Lombardo P, Rossi L, et al. Morning recall of verbal material depends on prior sleep organization.
Behav Brain Res 2000;112:159–63.
41. Walker M, Brakefield T, Morgan A, et al. Practice with sleep makes perfect: sleep dependent motor skill learning. Neuron 2002;35:205–11.
42. Fischer S, Hallschmid M, Elsner A, et al. Sleep forms memory for finger skills. PNAS 2002;99:11987–91.
43. Karni A, Meyer G, Rey-Hipolito C, et al. The acquisition of skilled motor performance: fast and slow experiencedriven changes in primary motor cortex. PNAS 1998;95:
861–8.
44. Plihal W, Born J. Effects of early and late nocturnal sleep on priming and spatial memory. Psychophysiology
1999;36:571–82.
45. Huber R, Ghilardi MF, Massimini M, et al. Local sleep and learning. Nature 2004;430(6995):78–81.
46. Ekstrand B, Sullivan M, Parker D, et al. Spontaneous recovery and sleep. J Exp Psychol 1971;88:142–4.
47. Plihal W, Born J. Memory consolidation in human sleep depends on inhibition of glucocorticoid release. Neuroreport 1999;10:2741–7.

335
48. Plihal W, Pietrowsky R, Born J. Dexamethasone blocks sleep induced improvement of declarative memory.
Psychoneuroendocrinol 1999;24:313–31.
49. Smith C, MacNeill C. Impaired motor memory for a pursuit rotor task following Stage 2 sleep loss in college students.
J Sleep Res 1994;3:206–13.
50. Kahn A, Van de Merckt C, Rebuffat E, et al. Sleep problems in healthy preadolescents. Pediatrics 1989;84:542–6.
51. Blum D, Kahn A, Mozin MJ, et al. Relation between chronic insomnia and school failure in preadolescents. Sleep Res
1990;19:194.
52. Link SC, Ancoli-Israel S. Sleep and the teenager. Sleep Res
1995;24a:184.
53. Gray EK, Watson D. General and specific traits of personality and their relation to sleep and academic performance. J Pers 2002;70:177–206.
54. Giannotti F, Cortesi F, Sebastiani T, Ottaviano S. Circadian preference, sleep and daytime behaviour in adolescence.
J Sleep Res 2002;11:191–9.
55. Carskadon MA, Wolfson AR, Acebo C, et al. Adolescent sleep patterns, circadian timing, and sleepiness at a transition to early school days. Sleep 1998;21:871–81.
56. Epstein R, Chillag N, Lavie P. Starting times of school: effects on daytime functioning of fifth-grade children in
Israel. Sleep 1998;21:250–6.
57. Lack LC. Delayed sleep and sleep loss in university students. J Am Coll Health 1986;35:105–10.
58. Meijer AM, Habekothe HT, Van Den Wittenboer GL. Time in bed, quality of sleep and school functioning of children.
J Sleep Res 2000;9:145–53.
59. Pilcher JJ, Ginter DR, Sadowsky B. Sleep quality versus sleep quantity: relationships between sleep and measures of health, well-being and sleepiness in college students.
J Psychosom Res 1997;42:583–96.
60. Trockel MT, Barnes MS, Egget DL. Health-related variables and academic performance among first-year college students: implications for sleep and other behaviors.
J Am Coll Health 2000;49:125–30.
61. Dotto L. Sleep stages, memory and learning. Can Med
Assoc J 1996;154:1193–6.
62. Sadeh A, Raviv A, Gruber R. Sleep patterns and sleep disruptions in school-age children. Dev Psychol 2000;36:
291–301.
63. Paavonen EJ, Aronen ET, Moilanen I, et al. Sleep problems of school-aged children: a complementary view. Acta
Paediatr 2000;89:223–8.
64. Kelly WE, Kelly KE, Clanton RC. The relationship between sleep length and grade-point average among college students. Coll Student J 2001;35:84–6.
65. Eliasson A, Eliasson A, King J, et al. Association of sleep and academic performance. Sleep Breath 2002;6:45–8.
66. Giannotti F, Cortesi F. Sleep Patterns and Daytime
Functions in Adolescents: An Epidemiological Survey of
Italian High-School Student Population. In: Carskadon MA, editor. Adolescent Sleep Patterns: Biological, Social, and
Psychological Influences. NewYork: Cambridge University
Press; 2002. p. 132–47.
67. Giannotti F, Cortesi F, Ottaviano S. Sleep, behavior and schoool functioning in school-aged children. Sleep Res
1997;26:197.
68. Bruni O, Antignani M, Innocenzi M, et al. Influence of sleep and temperament on school achievement. Sleep Res
1995;24:91.
69. Steenari MR, Vuontela V, Paavonen EJ, et al. Working memory and sleep in 6- to 13-year-old schoolchildren.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2003;42:85–92.

ARTICLE IN PRESS
336
70. Sadeh A, Gruber R, Raviv A. Sleep, neurobehavioral functioning, and behavioral problems in school-age children. Child Dev 2002;73:405–17.
71. Gau S-F, Song W-T. Sleep problems of junior high school students in Taipei. Sleep 1995;18:667–73.
72. Mercer PW, Merritt SL, Cowell JM. Differences in reported sleep need among adolescents. J Adol Health 1998;23:
259–63.
73. Jones K, Harrison Y. Frontal lobe function, sleep loss and fragmented sleep. Sleep Med Rev 2001;5:463–75.
74. Shin C, Kim J, Lee S, et al. Sleep habits, excessive daytime sleepiness and school performance in high school students. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2003;57:451–3.
75. Drake C, Nickel C, Burduvali E, et al. The Pediatric
Daytime Sleepiness Scale (PDSS): sleep habits and school outcomes in middle-school children. Sleep 2003;
26:455–8.
76. Howell AJ, Jahrig JC, Powell RA. Sleep quality, sleep propensity and academic performance. Percept Mot Skills
2004;99:525–35.
77. Salcedo Aguilar F, Rodrı
´guez Almonacid FM, Monterde
Aznar ML, et al. Habitos de sueno y problemas relaciona´
˜
dos con el sueno en adolescentes: relacion con el
˜
´ rendimiento escolar. Aten Primaria 2005;35:408–14.
78. Mitchell RB. Sleep-disordered breathing in children: are we underestimating the problem? Eur Respir J 2005;
25:216–7.
Ã79. Gozal D. Sleep-disordered breathing and school performance in children. Pediatrics 1998;102:616–20.
Ã80. Owens J, Spirito A, Marcotte A, et al. Neuropsychological and behavioral correlates of obstructive sleep apnea syndrome in children: a preliminary study. Sleep Breath
2000;4:67–77.
81. Goodwin JL, Kaemingk KL, Fregosi RF, et al. Clinical outcomes associated with sleep-disordered breathing in caucasian and hispanic children—the Tucson Children’s
Assessment of Sleep Apnea Study (TuCASA). Sleep 2003;
26:587–91.
82. Urschitz MS, Guenther A, Eggebrecht E, et al. Snoring, intermittent hypoxia and academic performance in primary school children. Am J Respir Crit Care Med
2003;168:464–8.
83. Kaemingk KL, Pasvogel AE, Goodwin JL, et al. Learning in children and sleep disordered breathing: findings of the
Tucson Children’s Assessment of Sleep Apnea (TuCASA)
Prospective Cohort Study. J Intern Neuropsychol Soc
2003;9:1016–26.
84. O’Brien LM, Mervis CB, Holbrook CR, et al. Neurobehavioral implications of habitual snoring in children.
Pediatrics 2004;114:44–9.
85. Montgomery-Downs HE, Crabtree VM, Gozal D. Cognition, sleep and respiration in at-risk children treated for obstructive sleep apnoea. Eur Respir J 2005;25:336–42.
86. Gozal D, Pope DW. Snoring during early childhood and academic performance at age 13–14 years years. Pediatrics 2001;107:1394–9.
87. Hoban TF. Sleep and its disorders in children. Semin
Neurol 2004;24:327–40.
88. Blaiss M. Current concepts and therapeutic strategies for allergic rhinitis in school-age children. Clin Ther 2004;
26:1876–89.
89. Copes K, Rosentswieg J. The effects of sleep deprivation upon motor performance of ninth-grade students. J Sports
Med Phys Fitness 1972;12:47–53.
90. Carskadon MA, Harvey K, Dement WC. Sleep loss in young adolescents. Sleep 1981;4:299–312.

G. Curcio et al.
91. Williams H, Lubin A, Goodnow J. Impaired performance with acute sleep loss. Psychol Monogr 1959;484:
1–26.
92. Horne JA. Human sleep, sleep loss and behaviour.
Implications for the prefrontal cortex and psychiatric disorder. Br J Psychiatry 1993;162:413–9.
93. Harrison Y, Horne JA. One night of sleep loss impairs innovative thinking and flexible decision making. Organ
Behav Hum Decis Process 1999;78:128–45.
94. Carskadon MA, Harvey K, Dement WC. Acute restriction of nocturnal sleep in children. Perc Mot Skills 1981;53:
103–12.
95. Pilcher JJ, Walters AS. How sleep deprivation affects psychological variables related to college students’ cognitive performance. J Am Coll Health 1997;46:121–6.
96. Randazzo AC, Muehlbach MJ, Schweitzer PK, et al.
Cognitive function following acute sleep restriction in children ages 10-14. Sleep 1998;21:861–8.
97. Randazzo AC, Schweitzer PK, Walsh JK. Cognitive function following 3 nights of sleep restriction in children ages 1014. Sleep 1998;21:s249.
98. Fallone G, Acebo C, Arnedt JT, et al. Effects of acute sleep restriction on behavior, sustained attention, and response inhibition in children. Percept Mot Skills 2001;93:213–29.
99. Fallone G, Seifer R, Acebo C, et al. Prolonged sleep restriction in 11- and 12-year-old children: effects on behavior, sleepiness and mood. Sleep 2000;23:A28.
Ã100. Sadeh A, Gruber R, Raviv A. The effects of sleep restriction and extension on school-age children: what a difference an hour makes. Child Dev 2003;74:444–55.
101. Drake CL, Roehrs TA, Burduvali E, et al. Effects of rapid versus slow accumulation of eight hours of sleep loss.
Psychophysiology 2001;38:979–87.
102. Hansen M, Janssen I, Schiff A, et al. The impact of school daily schedule on adolescent sleep. Pediatrics 2005;
115:1555–61.
103. Brown FC, Soper B, Buboltz WC. Prevalence of delayed sleep phase syndrome in university students. Coll Student
J 2001;35:472–5.
104. Ferrara M, De Gennaro L. How much sleep do we need?
Sleep Med Rev 2001;5:155–79.
105. Ohayon MM, Carskadon MA, Guilleminault C, et al. Metaanalysis of quantitative sleep parameters from childhood to old age in healthy individuals: developing normative sleep values across the human lifespan. Sleep
2004;27:1255–73.
106. Quirk KJ, Keith TZ, Quirk JT. Employment during high school and student achievement: longitudinal analysis of national data. J Ed Res 2001;95:4–10.
107. Schultz GF. Socioeconomic advantage and achievement motivation: important mediators of academic performance in minority children in urban schools. Urban Rev
1993;25:221–32.
108. Wimmer F, Hoffmann RF, Bonato RA, Moffitt AR. The effects of sleep deprivation on divergent thinking and attention processes. J Sleep Res 1992;1:223–30.
109. Harrison Y, Horne JA. Sleep deprivation affects speech.
Sleep 1997;20:871–7.
110. Harrison Y, Horne JA. Sleep loss impairs short and novel language tasks having a prefrontal focus. J Sleep Res
1998;7:95–100.
111. Harrison Y, Horne JA, Rothwell A. Prefrontal neuropsychological effects of sleep deprivation in young adults—a model for healthy aging? Sleep 2000;23:1067–73.
112. Drummond SP, Brown GG, Stricker JL, et al. Sleep deprivation-induced reduction in cortical functional

ARTICLE IN PRESS
Sleep loss, learning capacity and academic performance response to serial subtraction. Neuroreport 1999;10:
3745–8.
113. Thomas M, Sing H, Belenky G, et al. Neural basis of alertness and cognitive performance impairments during sleepiness. I. Effects of 24 h of sleep deprivation on

337

waking human regional brain activity. J Sleep Res 2000;
9:335–52.
114. Drummond SP, Brown GG, Gillin JC, et al. Altered brain response to verbal learning following sleep deprivation.
Nature 2000;403:655–67.

You May Also Find These Documents Helpful

  • Satisfactory Essays

    Based on my own personal experience, I agree with this article. I think that sleep deprivation poses a public health issue. Not getting enough sleep affects our grades, and how well we do in school. Along with some physical disadvantages, there are some psychological as well. In the article, it said, “...neural remodeling is important for learning and memory.” The quiet time can be good for strengthening or weakening certain neurons. Sleep is a significant thing for students to get so that they are mentally and physically ready to accomplish all of the hard tasks for the next day. I gathered some details from an article that stated, “There have been many studies proving that starting school later on in the day will promote students learning…

    • 364 Words
    • 2 Pages
    Satisfactory Essays
  • Better Essays

    directly associated with a student’s performance in school. The amount of sleep a pupil gets,…

    • 1332 Words
    • 6 Pages
    Better Essays
  • Good Essays

    As a second year Bachelors of Science student at the University of the Fraser Valley I have observed that juggling school, work and a social life can be quite difficult. It is interesting to see that many university students who have the appropriate sleep before an exam score moderately higher than those who lack proper sleep. Most students procrastinate and end up pulling "all nighters" the night before and exam praying that they will pass, but maybe a good night sleep would do just the same as staying up all night. I have chosen this topic because this issue effects me directly as a student and is a vastly relatable subject. Acquiring proper sleep is important in more aspects than just scoring higher on exams; it affects the health of an individual and the stability of the mind. Getting the right amount of sleep is an easy change that an individual can make to their lifestyle, which will provide various benefits to them.…

    • 1343 Words
    • 6 Pages
    Good Essays
  • Good Essays

    One of the main reasons early school start times are bad for teenagers is that it makes them seriously sleep deprived. When waking up around 6 or 7 a.m., students are lucky to get making 6 hours of sleep, when they’re supposed to get about 9 hours, because they stay up doing homework or another activity they were supposed to do the night before. In the article “School Start Times and Sleep”, the author explains this topic in more detail by saying “Evidence suggests that teenagers are indeed, seriously sleep…

    • 1273 Words
    • 6 Pages
    Good Essays
  • Good Essays

    If you had the choice, would you choose to sleep or go to school? Wouldn’t you want school to start later so you could get your nine hours? Well, it’s not as ideal as it seems. For the past few years, the Littleton Public Schools district, along with other school districts around the country, discussed the idea of the change of school start and end times. For the most part of this school year, the district has started to ask parents and students for their opinions and to come up with different solutions that will appeal to everyone. One of the biggest reasons that the district would need to change the times befalls the lack of sleep that students receive. Pamela McKeever, the leader of a study of adolescent sleep said that, "...research explains…

    • 1259 Words
    • 6 Pages
    Good Essays
  • Good Essays

    Starting school later have the most learning benefits." Researchers found that sleep deprivation disrupts connection between the amygdala and the medial prefrontal cortex and is critical because the medial prefrontal cortex itself regulates amygdala function. Sleep deprivation cause the amygdala to overreact to negative stimuli because it's disconnected from brain areas normally moderating its response. The benefit of getting sleep is amygdala regulation. Also a May 2012 study in Education Next looked at more than 146,000 middle schoolers in the Wake County district and found that pushing back their start times an hour increased standardized math and reading scores by 2 to 3 percentile points."(pg.1)This learning benefit is about what the attention span is…

    • 1002 Words
    • 5 Pages
    Good Essays
  • Good Essays

    High schools throughout the United States start their school day at times that are too early for students. Teenagers have a biological wake time, and when schools start early in the morning, it interferes with teens biological clock. According to a study done by Harvard Medical School, young adults need at least nine hours of sleep every night. The study also shows that the need for sleep is never greater in our lives than it is during adolescence, but yet the conflict between school and sleep is at it’s highest while teens are in high school. Many educators believe that if teens just went to bed earlier, they would…

    • 657 Words
    • 3 Pages
    Good Essays
  • Good Essays

    Research shows that kids who get more sleep on school nights automatically improve in all their classes. Even though, it may not seem like kids can improve in just one night, sleep can heal anything, kids that do not get enough sleep during the school year have a good chance of having, depression, attention-deficit, hyperactivity-disorder,…

    • 593 Words
    • 3 Pages
    Good Essays
  • Powerful Essays

    If only they went to bed earlier, concentration and attentiveness would improve. But in their report, the researchers say the schools are ignoring adolescent biology and are “systematically restricting the time available for sleep and causing severe and chronic sleep loss.” They're not alone. The chorus of voices in favor of schools starting later had been getting louder. In August, the Centers of Disease Control (CDC) issued findings that lay out a stark and sobering picture of how sleep deprivation harms teenagers, a common problem among high school students especially, adolescents who don't get enough sleep (CDC recommends 9-10 hours) have an increased risk of being overweight, suffering depression, and struggling academically.…

    • 1693 Words
    • 7 Pages
    Powerful Essays
  • Good Essays

    In that manner, it is true. “A National Sleep Foundation Poll showed that 59 percent of middle-school students and 87 percent of high-school students are getting less than the recommended 8.5 to 9.5 hours of sleep a night that they need for their physical and mental health”(Hungelmann). So, that meaning that students can’t focus in school so much because they are tired. “Students at later starting schools get more sleep, perform better academically”(“The Impact” 1). Parents love to see their children doing very well in school.…

    • 641 Words
    • 3 Pages
    Good Essays
  • Good Essays

    Sleep deprivation is a common struggle many adolescents confront daily in their lives. It is a chronic condition with severe negative consequences to the developing teenage body and its effects may be even more dire when combined with the stress of heavy schoolwork along with extracurricular activities. Professional doctors advise and recommend growing teenagers to sleep eight hours or more, however this proves to be rather very difficult with the interference of the standard school schedule starting early in the morning. Starting school at a later time may highly reduce the negative effects of sleep deprivation and possibly improve the conditions of students.…

    • 757 Words
    • 4 Pages
    Good Essays
  • Good Essays

    Academic Success

    • 950 Words
    • 3 Pages

    One university resource that I will be using to strengthen my writing and reading skills is the “Resources for Speakers of Other Languages”. I will use this because my primary language is not English and I have to learn to use correct verbs, adjectives, and other forms of English. My other university resource that I will be using is “Grammar Tutorial”. I will use this because I struggle with my grammar, and it will help me to learn how to use proper grammar and correct word ordering.…

    • 950 Words
    • 3 Pages
    Good Essays
  • Good Essays

    Mary A. Carskadon of Brown University, found that more mature adolescents had later circadian rhythm timing, based on melatonin secretions in saliva samples. This finding shows that melatonin secretion occurs at a later time in adolescents as they mature; thus, it is difficult for them to go to sleep earlier at night. The melatonin secretion also turns off later in the morning, which makes it harder to wake up early (Carskadon et al., 1998),” (Backgrounder). Melatonin is a hormone that helps control sleep and wake cycles (Melatonin). So this helps prove that going to bed early is not going to help teenagers not be tired the next day when they have to wake up early for school. There is even research done by Judith Owens proving that starting school later does result in students sleeping more, not just going to bed even later. “After the start time delay, mean school night sleep duration increased by 45 minutes, and average bedtime advanced by 18 minutes (95% confidence interval, 7-29 minutes [t423 = 3.36; P < .001]); the percentage of students getting less than 7 hours of sleep decreased by 79.4%, and those reporting at least 8 hours of sleep increased from 16.4% to 54.7%,”…

    • 397 Words
    • 2 Pages
    Good Essays
  • Good Essays

    Sacrificing Sleep

    • 1326 Words
    • 6 Pages

    How important is it for students to get a good night’s sleep? Sleep is the activity that occupies the most of our spare time more than anything. Next to food, water, and shelter, sleep is one of the most important necessities for human survival. Even though humans need sleep to function properly and in the extreme cases live, most people choose to ignore their need for sleep. In an article called Sacrificing Sleep For Study Time Doesn’t Make The Grade, author Michael Breus states students who stay up late studying are more likely to have academic problems the next day. All around the world there are students filled with procrastination. This procrastination has an effect on all of their assignments, and their studying time. Students also have other academic, and other extracurricular activities that take up a lot of their time, so it gets hard finding time to study. Some students will stay up very late for many hours the night before an exam hoping to get an exceptional grade, but sometimes not getting enough sleep can have a bad effect on their grade. Michael Breus wrote this article to address the importance of sleep. Breus talks about the amount of sleep teens consume, and how much sleep is needed every night. Breus also writes about what happens when students don’t get enough sleep. This paper will discuss why sleeping is important, the effects of good & bad sleep, and how much sleep is necessary everyday.…

    • 1326 Words
    • 6 Pages
    Good Essays
  • Good Essays

    Academic Success

    • 693 Words
    • 3 Pages

    Academic success is very important and almost essential to living. Without academic success there are not very many job opportunities that will lead to living a successful career or stable lifestyle. Likewise this is very essential and can be utilized by each and every individual. Starting now, I will tell you where I am in my academic career, where I have been and where I see myself by the fall of the 2012 semester.…

    • 693 Words
    • 3 Pages
    Good Essays

Related Topics