Top-Rated Free Essay
Preview

Acculturation, Biculturism and Marginalization: How Ethnic Identity Mediates Acculturation Stress Depending on Different Acculturation Attitudes of Family Related Migrants

Best Essays
4838 Words
Grammar
Grammar
Plagiarism
Plagiarism
Writing
Writing
Score
Score
Acculturation, Biculturism and Marginalization: How Ethnic Identity Mediates Acculturation Stress Depending on Different Acculturation Attitudes of Family Related Migrants
Acculturation, biculturism and marginalization: how ethnic identity mediates acculturation stress depending on different acculturation attitudes of family related migrants

Student: Flora June Lee

Student ID: N 7316429

Unit: PYB 404 Issues in social developmental psychology

Assignment: Literature Review

Lecturer: Mariann Martsin

Due Date: 06/05/13

Word Count: 3,544

* * Ross-Sheriff (2011) commented that international migration patterns have * changed as a consequence of broad social, political, economic, and environmental * trends and explained the causes of the driving forces were including war, * globalization, urbanization, and changing cultural norms regarding social roles and * responsibilities (Ross-Sheriff, 2011). With these complex trends of migration * patterns, Van Hear (2010) viewed migration as a process which was an integral part * of broader social transformations, but which also had its own internal dynamics with * other factors related to the migrating process, shaping social transformation in their * own way. Migration was also linked in complex ways to class, gender, generation, * ethnicity and other social factors, which were embodied in positions in home and host * communities, and in work and domestic relationships, all of which might be * transformed in the course of the migratory process (Van Hear, 2010). To understand this complex process of migration, especially under changing circumstances of one culture to another, it might be useful to build conceptual tools for understanding these transitory processes in migration studies and in social science more widely (Van hear, 2010). They also include mediating agents and transitions that need also to be accounted for, as well as intersections among class, gender, generation, ethnicity and other social ruptures as well as the main driving forces of migration (Van Hear, 2010).Of course there were other important concepts such as relations between time and space, between dynamics or processes and outcomes, and between structure and agency that needed to get attention (Van Hear, 2010).
However, it is impossible to discuss all different theoretical concepts involved in different types of migration process in the current limited study. Rather, this study tried to focus on psychological impacts such as ethnic identity and self-esteem on migration through acculturation processes particularly on family- related migration because different patterns of migration produced different communities and resulted in producing different migrant identities including varying levels of psychological distress (Jones, 2008). Further, few empirical studies have focused on migrant adults populations.

Most migrants identification related literatures tended to relate more for adolescents

or young children because identity formation might be particularly challenging in this

cohort, especially when the values and beliefs of their natal culture differed

significantly from those of the host society (Sodowsky, Kwan, & Pannu, 1995; as

cited in Farver, Narang, & Bhadha, 2002). Therefore, this study focused on

ethnic identity and self-identification issues of adult migrants’ themselves within a

family structure according to different theoretical models relevant to adaptation of

new cultures, because family was the basic instrument in the society (Nesdale,

Rooney, & Smith, 1997).

In fact, most cultural acquisition theories developed and evolved in 1990s

when international migration became a key issue in international politics at the

beginning of 1990s. As Castle (2002) argued that migration, development and

international relations were closely connected as migration was a major factor of

transformation for both sending and receiving countries for different types of

migrants (Castle, 2002). With this perspective, this study generally focused on

those migration culture acquisition theories developed in 1990 rather then looking at

current perspectives in the most recent literatures, which actually have evolved from

these original theories in 1990s (Castle, 2002). As the findings from these research

studies has had been mixed or sometimes contradictory, it was important to

understand the exact nature of the relationship between migrant ethnic identification

and the acculturation process both need to be specified and assessed properly with

coherent measurements and theoretical assumptions (Nesdale et al., 1997).

Important theoretical concepts: ethnic identity, acculturation, biculturism, and marginalisation. According to Phinney (1990; as cited in Farver, Narang & Bhadha., 2002), ethnic identity and acculturation were related but separate constructs. Ethnic identity involves an individual’s self-identification as a group member, a sense of belonging to an ethnic group, attitudes toward ethnic group of membership, and degree of ethnic group involvement (Farver et al., 2002). The term acculturation was defined in anthropology as those phenomena, which resulted when groups of individuals having different cultures came into continuous first-hand contact with subsequent changes in the original pattern of either or both groups (Redfield, Linton, & Herskovits, 1936; as cited in Birman, 1994). Although acculturation was a neutral term in this context (that is, change might take place in either or both groups), in practice, acculturation tended to induce more changes in one of the groups than in the other (Berry, 1990a; as cited in Berry, 1997) Berry (1997) argued that in all plural societies, cultural groups and their individual members, in both dominant and non-dominant situations, must deal with the issue of how to acculturate. According to Berry (1997), four acculturation strategies were introduced: assimilation, separation, marginalization, and integration.
When individuals do not wish to maintain their cultural identity and seek daily interaction with other new cultures, the assimilation strategy is defined. In contrast, when individuals place a value on holding on to their original culture, and at the same time wish to avoid interaction with others, then the separation is defined (Berry,
1997). When there is an interest in both maintaining one’s original culture, while in daily interactions with other groups, integration is the option; here, there is some degree of cultural integrity maintained, while at the same time seeking to participate as an integral part of the larger social network (Berry, 1997). Last, when there is little possibility or interest in cultural maintenance (often for reasons of enforced cultural loss), and little interest in having relations with others (often for reasons of exclusion or discrimination) then marginalization is defined (Berry, 1997). However, this acculturation categories model has been criticized methodologically (Rudmin, 2003, 2009; as cited in Schwartz et al., 2010) because all four of Berry’s categories were represented in the same way by creating the two by two matrix of acculturation categories between high and low. However, the cut off point between high and low was arbitrary and would differ across samples, making comparisons across studies difficult, resulting in the fact that all four categories existed and were equally valid (Rudmin, 2003; as cited in Schwartz et al., 2010) and suggesting that not all of Berry’s categories might exist in a given sample or population, and that some categories might have multiple subtypes (Schwartz et al.,
2010).
In particular, Berry (1997) viewed the term “biculturism” as referring to acculturation that involved the individual simultaneously in the two cultures that were in contact in integrative ways, which appeared to be a consistent predictor of more positive outcomes than the three alternatives of assimilation, separation, or marginalization. Berry and his colleagues (Sam & Berry, 1995) assessed the acculturation strategies of various immigrant groups in North America and the results showed that bicultural individuals experienced less acculturative stress, anxiety and fewer psychological problems significantly, while marginalized individuals suffered the most psychological distress, including problems with self-identification and cultural alienation, which adversely affected their self-esteem (Farver et al., 2002). However, Shiraev and Levy (2007) explained acculturative stress as a negative feeling that a marginalized person might experience as a distressing psychological reaction to any unfamiliar cultural environment based on the assumption that person and groups undergoing any social and cultural change should experience a certain amount of psychological distress. Generally, many early definitions of acculturation focused on exposure to two cultures simultaneously as a culture shock, which was a reactive state of specific pathology or deficit, rather than taking advantage of being bicultural (Berry & Annis, 1974; Shiraev et al., 2007). The validity of marginalization as an approach to acculturation by
Berry (1997) was also questioned (Del Pilar & Udasco, 2004; as cited in Schwartz et al., 2010). Schawartz et al. argued that the likelihood that a person would develop a cultural sense of self without drawing on either the heritage or receiving cultural contexts would be less likely to. The marginalization approach might be true only for the small segment of migrants who rejected both their heritage and receiving cultures
(Berry, 2006b). Indeed, studies using empirically based clustering methods have found small or nonexistent marginalization groups and scales that attempted to measure marginalization typically had poor reliability and validity compared with scales for the other categories (Cuellar, Arnold, & Maldonado, 1995; Unger et al.,
2002; as cited in Schwartz et al., 2010). As described earlier, the impact of migrant ethnic identity on psychological distress had comparatively diverse points of views if they were either negative or positive reactions, depending on different theoretical frames. For example, Social
Identity Theory (Tajfel & Turner, 2001) and Self-Categorization Theory (Turner,
1987) emphasized more on the importance to individuals of their identification with particular social groups. Social Identity Theory (Tajfel & Turner, 2001; as cited in
Yip, Gee, & Takeuchi, 2008) viewed a possible explanation for why ethnic identity might buffer the effects of discrimination. According to this theory, individuals chose from an array of possible social identity groups and, once those groups were chosen, individuals focused on the positive aspects of their in-group, which helped to boost their own esteem, suggesting that ethnic identity was more important to their overall identity (Yip et al., 2009). In contrast, if ethnicity was a central component of one 's identity, it might actually exacerbate the effects of discrimination, resulting in a greater negative impact on mental health, according to self-categorization theory (Turner, Hogg, Oakes,
Reicher, & Wetherell, 1987; as cited in Yip et al., 2008), suggesting that people should be more in tune with environmental cues that were relevant to an important aspect of their identity. That is, experiences of racial discrimination might be such a cue relevant to their ethnic identity. Indeed, research suggested that African American adults and adolescents who reported strong racial centrality were also more likely to report experiences of racial discrimination (Neblett, Shelton, & Sellers, 2004;
Sellers, Caldwell, Schmeelk-Cone, & Zimmerman, 2003; Sellers & Shelton, 2003; as cited in Yip et al., 2008). However, despite this emphasis by social theorists, they tended to forget the larger literature that involved with both ethnicity and the acculturation process (Liebkind, 1993; 1996; as cited in Nesdale, Rooney & Smith,
1997).
First of all, these different findings resulted from lack of inclusion of acculturation itself as a variable methodologically when acculturation was considered as a phenomenon in research designs (Sam and Berry, 2006). Without including acculturation as a variable, the explanations for human behavior similarities and differences across populations would remain incomplete (Sam et al., 2006). Second, a further criticism of the acculturation literatures was that the same two acculturation processes, and the same four-acculturation categories, characterized all migrants equally—regardless of the type of migrant, the countries of origin and settlement, and the ethnic group in question, according to Berry’s (1980) model and other similar approaches (Sam et al., 2006). Finally, the vast majority of studies in the acculturation literature have focused on behavioral acculturation (Schwartz et al., 2010). That is, most widely used acculturation measures included primarily (or only) items assessing language use and other cultural practices (e.g., Cuellar, Arnold, & Maldonado, 1995;
Stephenson, 2000; Szapocznik, Kurtines, & Fernandez, 1980; as cited in Schwartz et al., 2010) due to accepting the fact that cultural practices might provide only a fair proxy for cultural adaptation (Schwartz et al., 2010).
Theoretical frameworks for acculturation research Shiraev & Levy (2007) claimed that cross-cultural psychologists usually used three approaches to examine human activities in various cultural settings. They were the sociobiological approach, the sociological approach and eco-cultural approach
(Shiraev et al., 2007). In particular, the eco-cultural approach emphasized both the environment and the individual were seen as open and interchanging systems (Shiraev et al., 2007), introducing John Berry whom originally developed this theory further in contemporary cross-cultural psychology. Shiraev et al. (2007) also pointed out that specialists should to be able to explain how, why, and to what extent people differed from one another, when ecological, biological, cultural, and acculturation factors were identified and taken into consideration (Berry, J. W., Poortinga, Y. H., Segall, M.H.,
& Dasen. P.R.,1992; as cited in Shiraev et al., 2007). In related to the concerns pointed by Shiraev et al. (2007), Berry (1997) argued earlier there were important links between cultural context and individual behavioural development, demonstrating what happened to individuals who developed in one cultural context when attempting to re-establish their lives in another one through his acculturation research framework, by confirming the fact that acculturation was one of the most complex areas of research in cross-cultural psychology because the process involved more than one culture and in two distinct senses (Berry, 1997). According to Berry (1997), the concept of acculturation was employed to refer to the cultural changes resulting from different ethnic groups encountered, while the concepts of psychological acculturation and adaptation were employed to refer to the psychological changes and eventual outcomes that occur as a result of individuals experiencing acculturation. In another words, acculturation phenomena resulted from contact between two or more cultures and research on acculturation had to be comparative in order to understand variations in psychological outcomes that were the result of cultural variations in the two groups in contact
(Berry, 1997). In particular, this framework viewed the integration model of acculturation strategies the most desirable among other strategies, considering it the same as the biculturalism model (Berry, 1997). For example, Berry and his colleagues (Berry,
1980; Berry, J. W., Kim, U., Power, S., Young, M, & Bujaki, M., 1989; Berry, Kim,
Minde, & Mok, 1987; Sam & Berry, 1995 as cited in Farver et al., 2002) assessed the acculturation strategies of various immigrant groups in North America and the result showed that integration was the most psychologically adaptive attitude, arguing that integrated or bicultural individuals experienced less acculturative stress and anxiety and manifested fewer psychological problems than those who were marginalized, separated, or assimilated, whereas marginalized individuals suffered the most psychological distress, including problems with self-identification and cultural alienation, which also affected their self-esteem (Farver et al., 2002). However, Phinney, Cantu, and Kurtz (1997) found that American identity was associated with self-esteem only for non-Hispanic Whites, but not for other ethnic groups. These mixed results as explained above raised two issues in the acculturation literatures. First of all, cultural practices might offer only a substitute for cultural adpatations, as Portes and Rumbaut (2001 as cited in Schwartz et al., 2010) mentioned that many Asian American young adults in their sample were not proficient in their native languages, even though they still perceived their identification with their parents’ countries of origin and maintained many of their values (Schwarz et al., 2010). Secondly, most researchers on biculturism did not sufficiently define an accurate operational definition of biculturism so that interpretation of those research results were problematic (Birman, 1994). Indeed, one finding in the United States, was that self-identification as American was markedly higher in non-Hispanic Whites than in ethnic minority groups (e.g., Devos & Banaji, 2005; as cited in Schwartz et al., 2010) and many White Americans did not perceived themselves as members of an ethnic group (Schildkraut, 2007; as cited in Schwartz et al., 2010). In brief, different operational definition problems of acculturation arose from different theoretical models of acculturation regarding to their assumptions
(LaFromboise, Coleman, & Gerton, 1993). LaFromboise et al. (1993) assumed acculturation as one of substitutes among the biculturism models. Biculturism as defined in this theory was viewed as the alternation model, which implied an individual in two culture contacts could be competent in both cultures without losing one of the cultures’ competencies in distinct cultural contexts as alternation model, whereas, fusion model meant a blended cultural identity, consisting of a synthesis of aspects of both cultures (LaFromboise et al., 1993). However, Berry’s (1997) integrating approach of biculturism differed from the bicultural model (LaFromboise et al., 1993; as cited in Birman, 1994) and it emphasized more on the relationship between the two cultural groups based on its implicit assumption that one of two cultures were higher than the other within a single social structure (LaFromboise et al., 1993).
Benet-Martinez and colleagues found that “blended” bicultural individuals tended to report higher self-esteem and lower psychological distress than a marginal population (Chen et al., 2008 as cited in Schwartz et al., 2010) because the consistent availability of both cultural flows within the person’s everyday life increased the ease of activating the correct cultural schema in accordance with their environmental situations (Schwartz et al., 2010). In contrast, Tadmor, Tetlock, and Peng (2009) argued that the bicultural model considered those marginal individuals in positive ways, when there was little interest in cultural maintenance and little interest in having relations with others, suggesting positive aspects of being a marginal person might be (1) sharing his or her condition with others of the same original culture; (2) engaging in institutional practices that were shared by other marginal people; (3) experiencing no major frustration from social expectations; and (4) still perceiving himself or herself to be a member of a group (LaFromboise et al., 1993). According to Sam and Berry (2006), many studies of how migrants coped with intercultural contacts had discrepancies in the ways in which they were operationalized and measured. As no standardized or widely accepted acculturation measures existed, it was necessary to design a clear and explicit formulation of acculturation instrument in order to assess acculturation adequately (Sam et al., 2006).
Further Sam and Berry (2006) pointed out that most empirical studies widely used a self-report type of questionnaires that had been recognized limitations such as social desirability, emphasizing obtaining divergent validation by source of information other than the respondents’ reports. Therefore, it is vital to understand each theory within its specific assumptions and not to generalize across all situations regardless of their similar findings (LaFromboise et al., 1993). As this study discovered migrants’ acculturation processes so far within specific theoretical frameworks, literature findings in different research were mixed as to whether individuals could be highly acculturated and at the same time be strongly identified with their ethnic group
(Farver, Narang, & Bhadha., 2002). These confusing problems initially evolved because of the context in which migration arrangements and their acculturation processes were fundamentally transformed and increasingly uncertain due to globalization (Landolt & Da, 2005). Shiraev & Levy (2007) suggested a new approach to cross-cultural psychology in the twenty-first century, which was linked to the concept of globalization. Globalization was defined as a proliferation of cross-border flow and transnational networks due to new technologies of communication and transport that allowed frequent and multi-directional streams of people, ideas and cultural symbols
(Castle, 2010). Castle also argued that globalization leads to major changes in the character of international migration. In other words, the context for migrant incorporation has already changed radically and will continue to do so. The rise of multiculturalism itself rather than assimilation or biculturism is one sign of this, but is not the end of the story: new forms of identity and belonging go beyond multiculturalism (Castle, 2010). Even though there is limited empirical evidence for clear statements for globalization, there probably are highly cosmopolitan groups who feel at home everywhere such as global business and professional elites might correspond with this image. But most members of transnational communities fall between these extremes, and probably have contradictory and fluctuating identities
(Castle, 2002).
Conclusions

This study explored that a special case of cultural psychology was the study of

how individuals respond to situations where they were in transition between their

original culture and another that differed from it in some respects in terms of

acculturation, especially within a specific theoretical frame that could apply to

the specific situation (Adler & Gielen, 1994). There was no single theory widely

accepted by all social scientists to agree with the emergence and perpetuation of

international migration patterns in the world under globalization (Van Hear, 2010),

suggesting that the contemporary migrating context in which such migrating

arrangements were realized fundamentally kept transforming so that it became

increasingly uncertain (Landolt and Da, 2005)

Although the topic of cultural contact and individual’ change has attracted

considerable attention in contemporary cross-cultural psychology, the field has been

characterized by a lack of theoretical coherence, definitional problems with key

constructs, and single sample studies that limit the external validity of empirical

cross-cultural research (Ward and Kenney, 1994). As acculturation is a process which

takes place over time, and which results in changes both in the culture and in the

individual culture changes, it would be ideal o compare two sets of data are compared

over time using the same people. However, in practice, it is impossible in most

acculturation research settings (Sam et al., 2006). Instead, a common alternative to

longitudinal research is cross-sectional research in which a time-related variable, such

as length of residence or generational status can be used for the generalizability of

acculturation theories (Sam et al., 2006).

In general, researchers of migrating studies need to be aware that it is the selective nature of the sample that happens across all migrating research. That is, individuals who chose to migrate would be different from those who do not
(Sodowsky, G., Kwan, K., & Pannu, R., 1995; as cited in Farver et al., 1997). Finally, acculturation research generally focused on immigrants assumed to be permanently settled in their new host countries. As a result, the terms “migrants” or
“international migrants” referred to the same type of migrants collectively. Moreover, many countries were both sending and receiving countries for different types of migrants, or in the process of transition from one type to the other (Castel, 2002).
Therefore, where applicable, it is viable to design acculturation research studies classifying different types of migrants.

References
Adler, L. L., & Gielen, U.P. (Eds.). (1994). Cross-cultural topics in psychology. Westport: Praeger Publishers.
Berry, J.W. (1980). Social and cultural change. In Triandis, H. C., & Brislin, R. (Eds.). Handbook of cross-cultural psychology (pp. 211-279). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Berry, J. W., Kim, U., Power, S., Young, M, & Bujaki, M. (1989). Acculturation attitudes in plural societies. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 38, 185-206
Berry, (1990a). Psychology of acculturation. In Berman, J. (Eds.). Cross-cultural perspectives: Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (pp. 201-234). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
Berry, J. W. (1997). Immigration, acculturation, and adaption. Applied Psychology: An international review, 46(1), 5-68.
Berry, J. W., & Annis, R. (1974). Acculturation stress. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 5(4), 382-397.
Berry, J. W., Kim, U., Minde, T., & Mok, D. (1987). Comparative studies of acculturative stress. International Migration Review, 21, 591-511.
Berry, J. W., Poortinga, Y. H., Segall, M.H., & Dasen. P.R. (1992). Cross-cultural Psychology: Research and application. New York: Cambridge university Press.
Birman, D. (1994). Acculturation and human diversity in a multicultural society. In Trickett, E. J., Watts, R. J., & Birman D, (Eds.). Perspectives on people in context (pp. 261-284). San Franscisco: Jossey-Bass Inc.
Castele, S. (2002). Migration and community formation under conditions of globalization. The Center for Migration Studies of New York, 36(4), 1143- 1168.
Cuellar, I., Arnold, B., & Maldonado, R. (1995). Acculturation Rating Scale for Mexican Americans-II: a revision of the original ARSMA scale. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Science, 17, 275-304. doi: 10.1177/07399863950173001
DelPilar, J.A., & Udasco, J. O. (2004). Deculturation: Its lack of validity. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 10, 169-176. doi: 10.1037/1099- 9809.10.2.169
Devos, T., & Banaji, M. R. (2005). American = White? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 447-466. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.88.3.447
Farver, J. A., Narang, S. K., & Bhadha, B. R. (2002). East meets west: Ethnic identity, acculturation, and conflict in Asian Indian families. Journal of Family Psychology, 16(3), 338-350. doi: 10.1037//0893-3200.16.3.338
Jones, A. (2008). A silent but mighty river: the costs of women’s economic migration. Journal of Women in Culture and Society, 33(4), 761-807.
Landolt, D., & Da, W. W. (2005). The Spatially Ruptured Practices of Migrant Families: A Comparison of Immigrants from El Salvador and the People 's Republic of China. Current Sociology, 53, 625-652. doi: 10.1177/0011392105052719
LaFromboise., T., Coleman., H., & Gerton (1993). Psychological impact of biculturism: Evidence and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 114(3), 394-412.
Liebkind, K. (1993). Self-reported ethnic identity, depression and anxiety among youth Vietnamese refugees and their parents. Journal of Refugee Studies, 6, 25-39.
Neblett, E, Shelton, J. N., & Sellers, R. M. (2004). The role of racial identity in managing daily racial hassles. In Philogene, G. (Eds.). Race and identity: The legacy of Kenneth Clark. Washington DC: American Psychological Association Press.
Nesdale., D., Rooney., R., & Smith., L. (1997). Migrant ethnic identity and psychological distress. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 28(5), 569-588. doi: 10.1177/0022022197285004
Phinney, J.S. (1990). When we talk about American ethic groups, what do we mean? American Psychologist, 51, 918-917.
Phinney, J. S., & Ong, A. D. (2007). Conceptualization and measurement of ethnic identity: Current status and future directions. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 54, 271-281. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.54.3.271
Portes, A., & Rumbaut, R. G. (2001). Legacies: The story of the immigrant second generation. Berkerly: University of California Press
Redfield, R., Linton, R., & Herskovits, M. J. (1936) memorandum on the study of acculturation. American Anthropologist, 38, 149-152.
Ross-Sheriff, F. (2011). Global migration and gender. Journal of Women and Social

Works, 26(3), 233-238a. doi: 10.1177/0886109911417692

Rudmin, F.W. (2003). Critical history of the acculturation psychology of assimilation, separation, integration, and marginalization. Review of General Psychology, 7, 3-37. doi: 10.1177/01461670731197
Sam, D., & Berry, J. W. (1995). Acculturative stress among young immigrants in Norway. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 36, 10-24
Sam, D., & Berry, J. W. (2006). The Cambridge handbook of acculturation psychology [Electronic version]. Retrieved from http://www.qut.eblib.com.au.ezp01.library.qut.edu.au/patron.
Schildkraut, D. J. (2007). Defining American identity in the 21st century: How much “there” is there? Journal of Politics, 69, 597-615. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2508.2007.00562.x
Schwartz, S. J., Unger, J. B., Zamboanga, B. L., & Szapocznik, J. (2010). Rethinking the concept of acculturation: Implications for the theory and research. American Psychologist, 65(4), 237-251. doi: 10.1037/a0019330
Sellers, R. M., Caldwell, C. H., Schmeelk-Cone, K. H. & Zimmerman, M.A. (2003). Racial identity, racial discrimination, perceived stress, and psychological distress among African American young adults. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 44(3), 302-317.
Seller, R. M., & Shelton, R.M. (2000). The role of racial identity in perceived racial discrimination. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(5), 1079-1092.
Shiraev, E., & Levy, D. (2007). Cross-Cultural Psychology: Critical thinking and contemporary applications. Boston: Pearson Education Inc.
Sodowsky, G., Kwan, K., & Pannu, R., (1995). Ethnic identity of Asians in the United States. In J. Ponterotto (Ed.), Handbook of multicultural counseling (pp. 110- 130). Newbury Park: Sage.
Stephenson, M. (2000). Development and validation of the Stephenson Multigroup Acculturation Scale (SMAS). Psychological Assessment, 12, 77-88. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.12.1.77
Szapocznik, J., Kurtines, W., & Fernandez, T. (1980). Bicultural involvement and adjustment in Hispanic-American youths. International Journal of Interculture Relations, 4, 353-365.
Tadmor, C. T., Tedlock, P. E., & Peng, K. (2009). Acculturation strategies and integrative complexity: The congnitive implications of biculturism. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 40, 105-139. doi: 10.1177/0022022108326279
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (2001). An Integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In Hogg, M., & Abrams, D. (Eds.). The social psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 94-109). New York: Psychology Press.
Turner, J. C., Hogg, M.A., Oakes, P.J., Reicher, S.D. & Wetherell, M.S. (1987). Rediscovering the social group: A self-categorization theory. Oxford: Balckwell.
Unger, J.B., Gallagher, P., Shakib, S., Ritt-Olson, A., Palmer, P.H., & Johnson, C. A. (2002). The AHIMSA acculturation scale: A new measure of acculturation for adolescents in a multicultural society. Journal of Early Adolescence, 22, 225-251. doi: 10.1177/02731602022003001
Van Hear, N. (2010). Theories of migration and social change. Journal of Ethic and Migration Studies, 36(10), 1531-1536. doi: 10.1080/1369183X.2010.489359
Yip, T., G, C. G., & Takeuchi, D. T. (2008). Racial discrimination and psychological distress: The impact of ethnic identity and age among immigrant and United States-born Asian adult. Dev Psychol, 44(3), 787-800. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.44.3.787.

References: Adler, L. L., & Gielen, U.P. (Eds.). (1994). Cross-cultural topics in psychology. Berry, J.W. (1980). Social and cultural change. In Triandis, H. C., & Brislin, R. (Eds.) Berry, J. W., Kim, U., Power, S., Young, M, & Bujaki, M. (1989). Acculturation attitudes in plural societies 185-206 Berry, (1990a) Berry, J. W. (1997). Immigration, acculturation, and adaption. Applied Psychology: An international review, 46(1), 5-68. Berry, J. W., & Annis, R. (1974). Acculturation stress. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 5(4), 382-397. Berry, J. W., Kim, U., Minde, T., & Mok, D. (1987). Comparative studies of acculturative stress Berry, J. W., Poortinga, Y. H., Segall, M.H., & Dasen. P.R. (1992). Cross-cultural Psychology: Research and application Birman, D. (1994). Acculturation and human diversity in a multicultural society. In Trickett, E Castele, S. (2002). Migration and community formation under conditions of globalization Cuellar, I., Arnold, B., & Maldonado, R. (1995). Acculturation Rating Scale for Mexican Americans-II: a revision of the original ARSMA scale DelPilar, J.A., & Udasco, J. O. (2004). Deculturation: Its lack of validity. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 10, 169-176 Jones, A. (2008). A silent but mighty river: the costs of women’s economic migration. Journal of Women in Culture and Society, 33(4), 761-807. Landolt, D., & Da, W. W. (2005). The Spatially Ruptured Practices of Migrant Families: A Comparison of Immigrants from El Salvador and the People 's LaFromboise., T., Coleman., H., & Gerton (1993). Psychological impact of biculturism: Evidence and theory Liebkind, K. (1993). Self-reported ethnic identity, depression and anxiety among youth Vietnamese refugees and their parents Neblett, E, Shelton, J. N., & Sellers, R. M. (2004). The role of racial identity in managing daily racial hassles Nesdale., D., Rooney., R., & Smith., L. (1997). Migrant ethnic identity and psychological distress Phinney, J. S., & Ong, A. D. (2007). Conceptualization and measurement of ethnic identity: Current status and future directions Ross-Sheriff, F. (2011). Global migration and gender. Journal of Women and Social Works, 26(3), 233-238a Rudmin, F.W. (2003). Critical history of the acculturation psychology of assimilation, separation, integration, and marginalization Schildkraut, D. J. (2007). Defining American identity in the 21st century: How much “there” is there? Journal of Politics, 69, 597-615 Sellers, R. M., Caldwell, C. H., Schmeelk-Cone, K. H. & Zimmerman, M.A. (2003). Seller, R. M., & Shelton, R.M. (2000). The role of racial identity in perceived racial discrimination Shiraev, E., & Levy, D. (2007). Cross-Cultural Psychology: Critical thinking and contemporary applications Sodowsky, G., Kwan, K., & Pannu, R., (1995). Ethnic identity of Asians in the United States Stephenson, M. (2000). Development and validation of the Stephenson Multigroup Acculturation Scale (SMAS)

You May Also Find These Documents Helpful

  • Good Essays

    Doyle, Jamie Mihoko and Grace Kao. 2007. “Are Racial Identities of Multiracial stable? Changing Self Identification Among Single and Multiple Race Individuals.” Social Psychology Quarterly 70(4):405-423.…

    • 324 Words
    • 2 Pages
    Good Essays
  • Satisfactory Essays

    | |Pg.61 Schaefer, R. T. (2012). Racial and ethnic groups (13th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson |…

    • 290 Words
    • 2 Pages
    Satisfactory Essays
  • Satisfactory Essays

    References: Schaefer, R. T. (2012). Racial and ethnic groups (13th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall.…

    • 483 Words
    • 2 Pages
    Satisfactory Essays
  • Satisfactory Essays

    Analytical tool used to look at race as a socially constructed identity, where the content and importance of racial categories is determined by social, economic, and political forces…

    • 256 Words
    • 2 Pages
    Satisfactory Essays
  • Better Essays

    The graphic novel American Born Chinese (2006), by Gene Luen Yang, is a very modern and influential piece of work that can be compared to the short indie film Two Lies (1990), directed and written by Pamela Tom, which had preceded the novel by 16 years. These two different forms of work, both utilizing their ability to teach the audience, are used as powerful venues for the topic of identity crisis among the Asian people in a majority European American world. In the film, we have Mei and her family who are all having some trouble adjusting to their lives in Southern California but more specifically we have Mei and her trouble to understand her mother 's cause and intent for having undergone double eye-lid surgery. In ABC, we have our protagonist, Jin, who is having trouble fitting into his new school in San Francisco since he is one of the very few Asian admitted to the school. Another time line in the novel is the story of the monkey king who does anything to get rid of the fact that he is a monkey in order to fit into society. The third is the story of Danny, a European American who has trouble and often becomes embarrassed with his hyperbolic Chinese cousin, Chin-Kee. This character is first introduced by saying "Harro Amellica!" while Jin 's father, carrying giant Chinese take out container says "I 'll put your luggage into your room, Chin-Kee" (48). All three of these time line show our characters having some sort of shame or embarrassment to the fact that their own image or background is different from those around them.…

    • 2458 Words
    • 10 Pages
    Better Essays
  • Better Essays

    Eth/125 Week 7 Assignment

    • 1462 Words
    • 6 Pages

    Schaefer, R. T. (2006). Racial and Ethnic Groups (sixth ed.). Retrieved from University of Phoenix eBook Collection database.…

    • 1462 Words
    • 6 Pages
    Better Essays
  • Good Essays

    References: Schaefer, R. T. (2012). Racial and ethnic groups (13th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice…

    • 599 Words
    • 3 Pages
    Good Essays
  • Good Essays

    Olson, J and Beal, H (2010). The ethnic dimension in American history (4th ed.). Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell. ISBN: 9781405182515.…

    • 988 Words
    • 4 Pages
    Good Essays
  • Satisfactory Essays

    Reference: Schaefer, R. T. (2012). Racial and ethnic groups (13th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall.…

    • 1183 Words
    • 5 Pages
    Satisfactory Essays
  • Satisfactory Essays

    Discrimination Worksheet

    • 698 Words
    • 3 Pages

    Schaefer, R. T. (2012). Racial and ethnic groups (13th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall.…

    • 698 Words
    • 3 Pages
    Satisfactory Essays
  • Powerful Essays

    Biracial Identity

    • 3026 Words
    • 13 Pages

    Alba, R. D. (1990). Ethnic identity: The transformation of White America. NewHaven, CT:Yale University Press.…

    • 3026 Words
    • 13 Pages
    Powerful Essays
  • Good Essays

    Immigrants in the United States encounter many obstacles and conflicts while they struggle to absorb the new society from old culture. They struggle in two different languages, two different cultures, and two different people parts of the world. For some immigrants, it is easy to make an assimilation of new society. However, for some immigrants, it is difficult to assimilate to the new society because they already used to with their traditional home culture. The traditional home culture such as food, custom, values, norms are difficult to get rid of for some immigrants which make them difficult to live in the new society.…

    • 733 Words
    • 3 Pages
    Good Essays
  • Satisfactory Essays

    Racial Diversity

    • 570 Words
    • 3 Pages

    References: Schaefer, R. T. (2012). Racial and Ethnic Groups (13th ed.). Retrieved from The University of Phoenix eBook Collection.…

    • 570 Words
    • 3 Pages
    Satisfactory Essays
  • Powerful Essays

    Hyphenated Americans

    • 2367 Words
    • 10 Pages

    References: Steinberg, Stephen. The Ethnic Myth: Race, Ethnicity and Class in America. January 16, 2001. Boston, MA: Beacon Press.…

    • 2367 Words
    • 10 Pages
    Powerful Essays
  • Satisfactory Essays

    In conclusion, when framing the Asian American experience to be a unified yet diverse experience, the emphasis should be placed on increasing education to stress the diversity of immigrant experiences. Through the past treatment and current beliefs a majority of Americans hold, it is clear that all types of Asian Americans are considered to be the same and treated according to this mentality. Whether it is the naturalization act or the model minority myth, Asians are thought of as one group with little in-group variation. What needs to be done now is to increase the flexibility of the definition of Asian American to acknowledge the existence of distinct sub-categories such as Chinese-American, Vietnamese-American and Filipino-American and to…

    • 208 Words
    • 1 Page
    Satisfactory Essays