EDU622-0603A-01: Applying Learning Theories
Unit 1 IP
Dr. Trude Fawson
American Intercontinental University
June 17, 2006
Introduction
How do we come to know what we know? What is knowledge? These questions are important not only for epistemologists or philosophers who study knowledge, but, as well for those interested in the sciences and education. Whether knowledge is seen as absolute, separate from the knower and corresponding to a knowable, external reality or as seen as part of the knower and relative to the individual's experiences with his environment have far-reaching implications.
In ancient times, people believed that only God could provide glimpses of the real' world. During the Renaissance, the scientific method evolved as the perceived device of uncovering the truth'. Still, the modern view trusts in the ability of science to reveal the world'.
Early epistemological theories emphasized knowledge as being the awareness of objects that exist independent of any subject. Thus, objectivism saw knowledge as a representation of a real world that is thought of as existing, separate and independent of the knower; and this knowledge should be considered true only if it reflects that independent world. In contrast, the constructivist view argues that knowledge and reality do not have an objective or absolute value or, at the least, that there is no way of knowing this reality.
John Watson's 1913 Psychology as the Behaviorist Views It proposed abandoning Instrospectionist attempts to make consciousness a subject of experimental investigation focusing instead on behavioral manifestation of intelligence. B. F. Skinner later hardened behaviorist views to exclude inner physiological processes along with inward experiences as items of legitimate concern. Consequently, the "cognitive revolution" of the sixties styled itself as a revolt against behaviorism even though the processes cognitivism hypothesized would be public
References: Driscoll, M. (2005). Psychology of learning for Instruction. (3rd Ed). New York, USA. Ertmer, Peggy A., Newby, Timothy J. (1993). Behaviorism, Cognitivism, Constructivism: Comparing Critical Features from an Instructional Design Perspective Harris, Doug (1995). Cognitive Learning and Memory. Retrieved June 15, 2006 from http://comp.uark.edu/~jdharris/cogmem.html Kimble, Gregory A. (2000). Behaviorism and Unity in Psychology. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9(6), 208-212 Mager, R.F. (1984). Mager 's Tips on Instructional Objectives. Retrieved on June 13, 2006 from http://www2.gsu.edu/~mstmbs/CrsTools/Magerobj.html Murphy, Elizabeth (1997). Constructivism from Philosophy to Practice. Retrieved on June 10, 2006 from http://www.cdli.ca/~elmurphy/emurphy/cle.html Retrieved June 10, 2006 from http://www.personal.psu.edu/wlm103/insys581/ Riegler, A Science 6(1-3), 1-30. Retrieve on June 17, 2006 from http://www.univie.ac.at/constructivism/pub/fos/riegler/ Shuell, T 531-547. Retrieved on June 17, 2006 from the University of Oklahoma Library. Schowalter, John E. (1977). The Modification of Behavior Modification. Pediatrics, 59(1), 130-131 Van Oers, Bert, Hannikainen, Maritta (2001). Some Thought about Togetherness: An Introduction Veronin, Michael A., Patry, Roland (2002). Instructional Objectives: What They Are, What They Aren 't Watson, John B. (1913). Psychology as the Behaviorist Views It. Psychology Review, 20, 158-177