Top-Rated Free Essay
Preview

Artificial Intelligence

Powerful Essays
7017 Words
Grammar
Grammar
Plagiarism
Plagiarism
Writing
Writing
Score
Score
Artificial Intelligence
Network architecture of the long-distance pathways in the macaque brain

1 of 11

http://www.pnas.org/content/107/30/13485.full

Top
Abstract
Model: Deriving the Network
Description

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences www.pnas.org (/) >

Current Issue (/content/107/30.toc) >

vol. 107 no. 30 >

Results

Dharmendra S. Modha, 13485–13490

Discussion

(/content

Acknowledgments
Footnotes

Dharmendra S. Modha (/search?author1=Dharmendra+S.+Modha&sortspec=date&submit=Submit)
Raghavendra Singh (/search?author1=Raghavendra+Singh&sortspec=date&submit=Submit)

a,1

References
/107/30.toc)

and

This Issue
July 27, 2010 vol. 107 no. 30
Masthead (PDF)
(/content/107/30
/local/masthead.pdf)
Table of Contents
(/content
/107/30.toc)

b

Author Affiliations
Communicated by Mortimer Mishkin, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, MD, June 11, 2010 (received for review
March 27, 2009)

PREV ARTICLE
(/CONTENT/107/30
/13479.SHORT)

NEXT ARTICLE
(/CONTENT/107/30
/13491.SHORT)

Understanding the network structure of white matter communication pathways is essential for unraveling the mysteries of the brain 's function, organization, and evolution. To this end, we derive a unique network incorporating 410 anatomical tracing studies of the macaque brain from the Collation of Connectivity data on the Macaque brain (CoCoMac) neuroinformatic database. Our network consists of 383 hierarchically organized regions spanning cortex, thalamus, and basal ganglia; models the presence of 6,602 directed long-distance connections; is three times larger than any previously derived brain network; and contains subnetworks corresponding to classic corticocortical, corticosubcortical, and subcortico-subcortical fiber systems. We found that the empirical degree distribution of the network is consistent with the hypothesis of the maximum entropy exponential distribution and discovered two remarkable bridges between the brain 's structure and function via network-theoretical analysis. First, prefrontal cortex contains a disproportionate share of topologically central regions. Second, there exists a tightly integrated core circuit, spanning parts of premotor cortex, prefrontal cortex, temporal lobe, parietal lobe, thalamus, basal ganglia, cingulate cortex, insula, and visual cortex, that includes much of the task-positive and task-negative networks and might play a

Published online before print July
13, 2010, doi:
10.1073/pnas.1008054107
PNAS (Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences) July 27, 2010 vol. 107 no. 30 13485-13490
Classifications
Biological Sciences
Neuroscience
(/search?tocsectionid=Neuroscience& sortspec=date& submit=Submit)

special role in higher cognition and consciousness. neuroanatomy (/search?fulltext=neuroanatomy&sortspec=date&submit=Submit&andorexactfulltext=phrase) brain network (/search?fulltext=brain+network&sortspec=date&submit=Submit&andorexactfulltext=phrase) network analysis (/search?fulltext=network+analysis&sortspec=date&submit=Submit&andorexactfulltext=phrase) structural (/search?fulltext=structural&sortspec=date&submit=Submit&andorexactfulltext=phrase) functional (/search?fulltext=functional&sortspec=date&submit=Submit&andorexactfulltext=phrase)

In 1669, Nicolaus Steno (1) referred to white matter as “nature 's finest masterpiece.” White matter pathways in the brain mediate information flow and facilitate information integration and cooperation across functionally differentiated distributed centers of sensation, perception, action, cognition, and emotion. Uncovering the global topological regularities of the logical long-distance connections that are subserved by the physical white matter pathways is a key prerequisite to any theory of brain function, dysfunction, organization, dynamics, and evolution. Anatomical tracing in experimental animals has historically been the pervasive technique for mapping long-distance white matter projections (2–4). Given the resolution of anatomical tracing experiments, they

Abstract (/content/107/30
/13485.abstract)
Full Text (HTML)
Full Text (PDF) (/content/107/30
/13485.full.pdf+html)
Full Text + SI (Combined PDF)
(/content/107/30
/13485.full.pdf+html?withds=yes)
Figures Only (/content/107/30
/13485.figures-only)
Supporting Information (/content
/107/30/13485/suppl
/DCSupplemental)

typically furnish data at a macroscale of cortical areas or, more generally, brain regions. The associated network description* models brain regions as vertices and the presence of reported long-distance connections as directed edges between them.

12/5/2013 10:50 PM

Network architecture of the long-distance pathways in the macaque brain

http://www.pnas.org/content/107/30/13485.full

The most well-known network of the macaque monkey visual cortex consists of 32 vertices and 305 edges (2).
Other networks of the macaque cortex consist of 70 vertices and 700 edges (5) and 95 vertices and 2,402 edges (6). The largest network of the cat cortex has 95 vertices and 1,500 edges (7). Network-theoretical
Top
analyses have uncovered a number of remarkable insights: distributed and hierarchical structure of cortex (2);
Abstract
topological organization of cortex (8); indeterminacy of unique hierarchy (9); functional small-world

characteristics, optimal set analysis, and multidimensional scaling (10); small-world characteristics (11);

Model: Deriving the Network nonoptimal component placement for wire length (6); structural and functional motifs (12); and hub
Description

identification and classification (13). However, even the largest previous network (6) completely lacks edges

Results corresponding to corticosubcortical and subcortico-subcortical long-distance connections and has significant

gaps even among corticocortical long-distance connections (SI Appendix, Fig. S1 (/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073
/pnas.1008054107/-/DCSupplemental/sapp1.pdf)).

Discussion

Acknowledgments

To gain a better understanding of the structure and organization of the brain, a network spanning the entire brain would be extremely useful. Such a network will be an indispensable foundation for clinical, systems,
Footnotes
cognitive, and computational neurosciences (14). No such network has been reported. We undertake the
References of constructing, visualizing, and analyzing such a network. Our network opens the door to the challenge 2 of 11

application of large-scale network-theoretical analysis that has been so successful in understanding the
Internet (15), metabolic networks, protein interaction networks (16), various social networks (17), and searching the World-Wide Web (18, 19).

Collation of Connectivity data on the Macaque brain (CoCoMac), a seminal contribution to neuroinformatics, is a publicly available database (20–22). Conscientiously and meticulously, the database curators have collated and annotated information on over 2,500 anatomical tracer injections from over 400 published experimental studies. CoCoMac is an objective, coordinate-independent collection of annotations that captures two relationships between pairs of brain regions, where each brain region refers to cortical and subcortical subdivisions as well as to combinations of such subdivisions into sulci, gyri, and other large ensembles. The first relationship is connectivity—whether a brain region in one study projects to another region in (possibly) a different study.
There are 10,681 connectivity relations.† The second relationship is mapping—whether a brain region in one study is identical to, a substructure of, or a suprastructure of another region in (possibly) a different study.
There are 16,712 mapping relations. Unfortunately, because of a multiplicity of brain maps, divergent nomenclature, boundary uncertainty, and differing resolutions in different studies, mapping relations are often conflicting and connectivity information is typically scattered across related brain regions. The situation is aptly described by Van Essen (23): “Our fragmentary and rapidly evolving understanding is reminiscent of the

(/external-ref?tag_url=http://w title=Network%20architecture%2 distance%20pathways%20in%2
%20%2830%29%3A%2013485+
link_type=FACEBOOK)
(/external-ref?tag_url=http://w
title=Network%20architecture%2 distance%20pathways%20in%2 %20%2830%29%3A%2013485+

(/external-ref?tag_url=http://w title=Network%20architecture%2 distance%20pathways%20in%2
%20%2830%29%3A%2013485+
link_type=CITEULIKE)
(/external-ref?tag_url=http://w
title=Network%20architecture%2 distance%20pathways%20in%2 %20%2830%29%3A%2013485+ link_type=DEL_ICIO_US) (/external-ref?tag_url=http://w title=Network%20architecture%2 distance%20pathways%20in%2
%20%2830%29%3A%2013485+
(/external-ref?tag_url=http://w title=Network%20architecture%2 distance%20pathways%20in%2
%20%2830%29%3A%2013485+
link_type=MENDELEY)

situation faced by cartographers of the earth 's surface many centuries ago, when maps were replete with uncertainties and divergent portrayals of most of the planet 's surface.” Consolidating connectivity information by merging logically equivalent brain regions and aggregating their connectivity is a necessary prerequisite to any network-analytical study. Further, it is desirable to place the merged brain regions into a coherent, unified, hierarchical brain map that recursively partitions brain and its constituents into progressively smaller physical regions.‡ The brain map can provide a natural frame of reference within which to correlate, aggregate, and understand various merged brain regions. Conceptually, merging brain regions and extracting a hierarchy can be carried out according to logical and formal calculus developed by CoCoMac curators (20–22, 24). In

SUBMIT AN ARTICLE
(HTTP://WWW.PNASCENTRAL.ORG)

practice, the tasks are made formidable by a number of factors. For example, (i) there are partially overlapping brain regions (SI Appendix, Fig. S5 (/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1008054107/-/DCSupplemental
/sapp1.pdf)); (ii) there are direct conflicts between mapping relations (SI Appendix, Fig. S3 (/lookup/suppl
/doi:10.1073/pnas.1008054107/-/DCSupplemental/sapp1.pdf)); (iii) there are implied indirect conflicts that are far too numerous and inherently insidious (SI Appendix, Fig. S3 (/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1008054107
/-/DCSupplemental/sapp1.pdf)); and (iv) there are errors and omissions in the underlying database, which itself is large. Although it is difficult to define a formal metric against which a single hierarchical brain map can be defensibly constructed, reassuringly, any hierarchical brain map built on the same set of merged regions will at most affect the resolution of the network-theoretical analysis. In this study, we have constructed one hierarchical brain map, at the highest resolution that the data can meaningfully support, toward our goal of network analysis (SI Appendix (/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1008054107/-/DCSupplemental/sapp1.pdf)).
The entire set of merged brain regions and our hierarchical brain map are explicitly detailed in the multipage
SI Appendix, Table S1 (/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1008054107/-/DCSupplemental/sapp1.pdf) to provide complete transparency and to permit future additions, deletions, and modifications as data with finer resolution become available.

12/5/2013 10:50 PM

Network architecture of the long-distance pathways in the macaque brain

http://www.pnas.org/content/107/30/13485.full

SI Appendix, Fig. S6 (/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1008054107/-/DCSupplemental/sapp1.pdf) visualizes our hierarchical brain map. It can be seen that the brain is divided into cortex, diencephalon, and basal ganglia, which are themselves divided into smaller regions, such as temporal lobe, frontal lobe, parietal lobe,

Top

occipital
Abstract lobe, insula, and cingulate cortex. With the brain regions in the hierarchical brain map as vertices, our network contains 6,602 edges, wherein an edge encodes the presence of long-distance connection between
Model: Deriving the Network corresponding brain regions.Fig. 1 displays the network on the hierarchical brain map, where each edge is
Description

visualized by a spline curve. Visualizing 6,602 edges directly leads to a highly cluttered figure in which no

details
Results

are discernible (SI Appendix, Fig. S17A (/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1008054107
/-/DCSupplemental/sapp1.pdf)). To improve clarity, splines with a common origin or destination are bundled

Discussion algorithmically (25) (SI Appendix and SI Appendix, Figs. S16 and S17 (/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073

/pnas.1008054107/-/DCSupplemental/sapp1.pdf)). The figure succinctly captures many aspects of the

Acknowledgments

cumulative contribution of a whole community of neuroanatomists over the past half century into a single

illustration.
Footnotes
References

3 of 11

Fig. 1.
Macaque brain long-distance network. Each vertex of the network corresponds to a brain region in the hierarchical brain map of SI Appendix, Fig. S6 (/lookup/suppl
/doi:10.1073/pnas.1008054107/-/DCSupplemental
/sapp1.pdf), and each edge encodes the presence of long-distance connection between corresponding brain

(13485/F1.expansion.html)

regions. Edges are drawn using algorithmically bundled splines (25). SI Appendix, Tables S2 and S3 (/lookup
/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1008054107/-/DCSupplemental

/sapp1.pdf) provide a summary of the number of edges in
In this page (13485/F1.expansion.html) major corticocortical and corticosubcortical subnetworks.
View larger version:

A color wheel is used for better discrimination amongst
In a new window (13485/F1.expansion.html) brain regions. For the
Download as PowerPoint Slide (/powerpoint/107/30/13485/F1)leaf brain regions in the two

outermost circles, the color wheel is rotated by 120° and
240°. The edges are drawn in black. SI Appendix, Table
S1 (/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1008054107/-/DCSupplemental/sapp1.pdf) enumerates the entire hierarchical brain map and provides a complete index to acronyms of the brain regions; it has been color-coded for wider accessibility.

The long distance network dataset consists of three text files: Macaque_LongDistance_Network.nameslist
(/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1008054107/-/DCSupplemental/sd01.txt),
Macaque_LongDistance_Network_connectivity.edgelist
(/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1008054107
/-/DCSupplemental/sd02.txt), and Macaque_LongDistance_Network_mapping.edgelist
(/lookup/suppl
/doi:10.1073/pnas.1008054107/-/DCSupplemental/sd03.txt). The files are publicly available and are described in SI Appendix (/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1008054107/-/DCSupplemental/sapp1.pdf).
Our network is (i) comprehensive in that it incorporates every study included in CoCoMac; (ii) consistent in that every edge can be tracked back to an underlying tracer study; (iii) concise in that identical brain regions
(e.g., V1, 17, striate cortex) are merged and their connectivity is aggregated, thus reducing brain regions to
383 from 6,877 in the original database; (iv) coherent in that brain regions are organized in a unified hierarchical parcellation or brain map; and, finally, (v) colossal in that it is roughly three times larger than the largest previous such network (6) (compare Fig. 1 with SI Appendix, Fig. S1 (/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073
/pnas.1008054107/-/DCSupplemental/sapp1.pdf)).
The comprehensiveness of our network is underscored by the fact that it contains logical subnetworks corresponding to a number of important physical fiber systems, namely, the visual system (2); dorsal-ventral pathways (3); thalamocortical relays (26); and numerous corticocortical, corticosubcortical, and subcorticosubcortical fiber systems (4). The brain regions involved in these fiber systems are enumerated in SI
Appendix, Table S4 (/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1008054107/-/DCSupplemental/sapp1.pdf), and the corresponding subnetworks are illustrated in SI Appendix, Figs. S18–21 (/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073
/pnas.1008054107/-/DCSupplemental/sapp1.pdf). It is important to note that strength, trajectory, and laminar source/target of projections are missing from our network, which only encodes the presence of connections.

12/5/2013 10:50 PM

Network architecture of the long-distance pathways in the macaque brain
Preliminary

analysis

(SI

Appendix

http://www.pnas.org/content/107/30/13485.full

(/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1008054107/-/DCSupplemental

/sapp1.pdf)) confirms that the network is sparse, reciprocal, and small-world (27, 11) and reveals that the
Top
network has the proverbial six degrees of separation (28). As our main contributions, we first characterize the degree Abstract distribution, that is, the probability distribution of the number of connections that each brain region makes. Second, we study topologically central regions and subnetworks in the brain and, in the process,
Model: Deriving the Network reveal two remarkable anatomical substrates of behavior via network-theory and web-searching algorithms.
Description
Results
Discussion

In a network, degree of a vertex is the total number of edges that it touches. The tail behavior of the frequency distribution of degrees is a key signature of how
Acknowledgments

connectivity is spread among vertices. A scale-free network follows a power law; that is, asymptotically, the
−γ
for some positive power γ.

Footnotes probability that a vertex is connected with k other vertices is proportional to k

Scale-free networks naturally arise via mechanisms of growth and preferential attachment (29). For an exponential network, asymptotically, the probability that a vertex is connected with k other vertices is

References

4 of 11

−k/λ

proportional to e
, for some positive constant λ. Exponential networks can arise via random network evolution (30) or via a mechanism that hinders preferential attachment (31), such as the cost of adding links to the vertices or the limited capacity of a vertex. The World-Wide Web, the Internet (15), some social networks, and the metabolic networks are all scale-free (16), whereas power grids, air traffic networks, and collaboration networks of company directors (31, 16) are all exponential.
A simple but fundamental unanswered question is whether the degree distribution of the brain network is scale-free, exponential, or neither? In related work, Humphries et al. (32) reported that the brainstem reticular formation is not a scale-free network. For the smaller brain networks, Sporns and Zwi (11) did not find evidence for power law distribution but left open the possibility that a large-scale network may uncover such structure. Further confusing the matter, Eguíluz et al. (33) found that functional networks of the human brain are scale-free, but Achard et al. (34) argued that at the level of resting state networks between cortical areas, these same networks are not scale-free. Restricted by the small size of available networks, Kaiser et al. (35) pursued an indirect approach based on simulated lesion studies (36) and concluded that “cortical networks are affected in ways similar to scale-free networks concerning the elimination of nodes or connections.
However, a direct comparison of degree distributions has been impossible.”
Armed with our network, we provide a fresh perspective on the controversy. Based on the recipe for analyzing power law distributions in the study by Clauset et al. (37), Fig. 2A demonstrates that the maximum likelihood scale-free hypothesis is untenable. Fig. 2B and SI Appendix, Fig. S22 (/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073
/pnas.1008054107/-/DCSupplemental/sapp1.pdf) demonstrate that over the finite range of available data, the maximum entropy exponential distribution fits the data well. It is noteworthy that for the 302-neuron network in the worm Caenorhabditis elegans (38), the tail of the degree distribution is also well approximated by exponential decays (31).

Fig. 2.
Our network is directed, meaning that each edge is an ordered pair of vertices. By keeping the connectivity but removing direction, we created the undirected version of our network that has 383 vertices and 5,208 edges. The undirected network has an average degree of λ = 27.2.
Following Keller (39), we analyze the behavior of the empirical complementary cumulative degree distribution
(also known as survival function), which is drawn using circles on both of the above plots. The dashed line in the

(13485/F2.expansion.html)

top log-log plot shows the complementary cumulative
In this page (13485/F2.expansion.html) distribution of the maximum likelihood power law fit,
−3.15
∼x
, x ≥ 33, which was derived using the software
In a new window (13485/F2.expansion.html)
View larger version:

provided with Clauset
Download as PowerPoint Slide (/powerpoint/107/30/13485/F2)et al. (37). Moreover, the P value is extremely small (

); hence, the maximum likelihood

power law hypothesis is rejected (37, box 1). The dashed line in the bottom log-linear plot shows the complementary cumulative distribution of the maximum
−1

entropy exponential distribution fit, λ

exp(−x/λ), over the entire range of data. The bottom plot is

12/5/2013 10:50 PM

Network architecture of the long-distance pathways in the macaque brain

http://www.pnas.org/content/107/30/13485.full

also shown using the linear-linear scale in SI Appendix, Fig. S22 (/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073
Top

/pnas.1008054107/-/DCSupplemental/sapp1.pdf). These plots suggest that the hypothesis of the maximum entropy exponential distribution is consistent with the data.

Abstract

We have seen that vertices in our network have differing

Model: Deriving the Network degrees of connectivity. We now introduce a number of widely studied metrics of topological centrality that
Description

take into account how vertices are interconnected.

Results

In- and out-degrees, respectively, are direct measures of how much information a vertex receives and sends.

For each
Discussion vertex, define out-closeness as its average shortest path to every other vertex and its in-closeness as the average shortest path to it from every other vertex (40). For each vertex, define betweenness centrality as the number of shortest paths that pass through it (41, 40). PageRank was developed in the context of Web

Acknowledgments

searching to find how often a vertex will be visited during random network traversal (18). Betweenness centrality and PageRank, which take both in- and out-connections into account, measure the efficacy of

Footnotes

Referencesin information intermediation. Hubs and authorities were also developed in the context of Web vertices 5 of 11

searching, and are defined relative to each other. They are recursively, circularly, and iteratively computed: A good hub links to many good authorities, and a good authority is one that is linked to by many good hubs (19).
Hubs distribute information, whereas authorities aggregate information.
Table 1 shows the top 10 brain regions according to the above metrics of topological centrality. Roughly, 70% of the top 10 regions according to in-degree, in-closeness, and authorities reside primarily in prefrontal cortex
(32, 46, 12o, 12l, 11, 14, 8A, 8B, 14, 9), suggesting that it serves as an integrator of information.

View this table:

Table 1.

In this window (13485/T1.expansion.html) 10 brain regions according to several metrics of
Top
In a new window (13485/T1.expansion.html)

topological centrality for our network

The top out-degree, out-closeness, and hub regions are distributed across prefrontal cortex (46, 9, 13, 13a,
45, 12, and 32), temporal lobe (TH, TF, and TE), parietal lobe (LIP and PGm), cingulate cortex (24 and 23), occipital lobe (V2), and thalamus (PM#3), with prefrontal cortex claiming 40% of the top 10 regions. This indicates that prefrontal cortex may also serve as a distributor of information. The top 10 regions according to betweenness and PageRank are distributed across prefrontal cortex (46, 13a, 32, 13, PS, 12o, 12l, and 11), temporal lobe (TF, PIT, and 36r), cingulate cortex (24 and 23c), parietal lobe (LIP), and thalamus (MD), with roughly half of the top regions residing in prefrontal cortex. Together, in a precise, quantitative, and multidimensional fashion, these facts strengthen the hypothesis that prefrontal cortex is an efficient intermediary of information serving both as an integrator and a distributor.
Is the topological centrality of prefrontal cortex an artifact of prefrontal regions being studied more often? Our investigation (SI Appendix, Figs. S23–28 (/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1008054107/-/DCSupplemental
/sapp1.pdf)) did not find that prefrontal cortex (and its subregions) was studied more often than other brain regions in CoCoMac data, nor did it find a correlation between how often a region is studied and its degree.
On the other hand, as expected, SI Appendix, Fig. S29 (/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1008054107
/-/DCSupplemental/sapp1.pdf) finds that degree is correlated with centrality. Together, these facts imply that topological centrality of prefrontal cortex is not attributable to it being studied more often.
Topological centrality indicates that some vertices are more special than others. A logical ensuing question is whether the brain network contains special subnetworks.
Now, we demonstrate that the brain network indeed contains a special subnetwork that captures its topological essence.
Core decomposition is a computationally efficient algorithm (17) that recursively peels off the least connected vertices to reveal progressively more closely connected subnetworks. In the first step, the algorithm recursively peels off all vertices with only one edge until only vertices with at least two edges remain. In the second step, the algorithm recursively peels off all the vertices with only two edges until only vertices with at least three edges remain. The algorithm continues in like manner until all vertices are peeled off. Each peeling step defines a core. Each core is a subset of the previous core; hence, the cores constitute a nested hierarchy
(SI Appendix, Fig. S31 (/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1008054107/-/DCSupplemental/sapp1.pdf)).
Progressing along the hierarchy yields successive cores that are ever more tightly interconnected. The last or

12/5/2013 10:50 PM

Network architecture of the long-distance pathways in the macaque brain

http://www.pnas.org/content/107/30/13485.full

the innermost core is the top of this hierarchy and constitutes a topologically central subnetwork.
We found the innermost core for the undirected version of our network (Fig. 3), and it turned out to be a remarkable topological structure. The innermost core is deeply nested (SI Appendix, Fig. S31 (/lookup/suppl

Top

/doi:10.1073/pnas.1008054107/-/DCSupplemental/sapp1.pdf)), such that each vertex in the innermost core
Abstract
touches at least 29 other vertices in the innermost core. The innermost core has 122 vertices. Let us refer to
Model: Deriving the Network vertices as the crust. There are 2,872 edges from the innermost core to itself, 1,707 the set of remaining 261
Description

edges from the crust to the innermost core, and 1,230 edges from the innermost core to the crust. There are

only 793 edges from the crust to itself. Thus, 88% of all edges either originate or terminate in the innermost
Results
core, although it contains only 32% of the vertices. The longest shortest path (namely, diameter) for the
Discussion core is only 4, whereas for the overall network, it is significantly higher, namely, 6. Similarly, the innermost average shortest path between any two vertices in the innermost core is only 1.95, whereas for the overall

Acknowledgments

network, it is significantly higher, namely, 2.62. Further, the innermost core contains the vast majority of

topological central vertices in Table 1 (SI Appendix, Fig. S32 (/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1008054107
Footnotes
/-/DCSupplemental/sapp1.pdf)). Thus, the innermost core is a central subnetwork that is far more tightly
References than the overall network, information likely spreads more swiftly within the innermost core than integrated 6 of 11

through the overall network, and the overall network communicates with itself mainly through the innermost core. Fig. 3.
Innermost core for the undirected version of our network.
The innermost core is a central subnetwork that is far more tightly integrated than the overall network.
Information likely spreads more swiftly within the innermost core than through the overall network, the overall network communicates with itself mainly through the innermost core, and the innermost core contains major
(13485/F3.expansion.html)

components of the task-positive and task-negative networks derived via functional imaging research (43).

View larger version:
In this page (13485/F3.expansion.html)
In a new window (13485/F3.expansion.html)
Download as PowerPoint Slide (/powerpoint/107/30/13485/F3)

Although the innermost core is structurally interesting, it is functionally even more intriguing. The innermost core spans premotor and prefrontal cortex (42 regions), temporal lobe (23 regions), parietal lobe (16 regions), thalamus (15 regions), basal ganglia (12 regions), cingulate cortex (7 regions), insula (6 regions), and V4 in visual cortex. SI Appendix (/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1008054107/-/DCSupplemental/sapp1.pdf) enumerates all brain regions in the innermost core. Three decades of functional brain imaging research in humans has culminated in the definition of two dynamically anticorrelated functional networks: a task-positive network activated during goal-directed performance and a task-negative network implicated in self-referential processing (43). Assuming a plausible set of homologies between human and macaque cortical organization,§ we found that the innermost core contains major components of both of these networks (SI Appendix (/lookup
/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1008054107/-/DCSupplemental/sapp1.pdf) and SI Appendix, Fig. S33 (/lookup/suppl
/doi:10.1073/pnas.1008054107/-/DCSupplemental/sapp1.pdf)). The innermost core constitutes the anatomical substrate that mediates temporally coordinated correlations within each network and anticorrelations between the networks and upholds physiological correlates underlying behavior.
Given the structural and functional centrality of the innermost core, it is natural to ask if it is sensitive to changes in the network. Quite reassuringly, precise analysis has revealed that the innermost core cannot change dramatically, given modest additions or deletion of edges in the network (SI Appendix, Tables S5 and
S6 (/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1008054107/-/DCSupplemental/sapp1.pdf)); hence, it is an extremely stable and robust signature of the network.

We have collated a comprehensive, consistent, concise, coherent, and colossal network spanning the entire

12/5/2013 10:50 PM

Network architecture of the long-distance pathways in the macaque brain

http://www.pnas.org/content/107/30/13485.full

brain and grounded in anatomical tracing studies that is a stepping stone to both fundamental and applied research in neuroscience and cognitive computing (14). What was previously scattered across 410 papers,
10,681 connectivity relations, and 16,712 mapping relations and limited to neuroanatomists specializing in the
Top
wetware of the experimental animals is now unified and accessible to network scientists who can unleash their
Abstract
algorithmic software toolkits (20–22).
Model: Deriving the Network
We have begun to uncover remarkable global topological regularities of the network. The maximum entropy
Description

exponential distribution

Results
Discussion
characterizes

the degree distribution of the network surprisingly well. Prefrontal cortex claims a

disproportionately large share of topologically central brain regions according to a variety of ranking schemes, and thus serves as both an integrator and a distributor of information in the brain. We have found a deeply

Acknowledgments

nested
Footnotesand tightly integrated core circuit spanning the entire brain that contains both the task-positive and task-negative networks. Assuming homology, it is indeed reassuring that the core circuit computed using
References data from a half century of anatomical tracing data in nonhuman primates corresponds so well with structural 7 of 11

3 decades of behavioral imaging research in humans. This hints at an evolutionarily preserved core circuit of the brain that may be a key to the age-old question of how the mind arises from the brain.

We thank four anonymous reviewers for a number of constructive suggestions that greatly improved and expanded our original submission. We thank curators of the CoCoMac databases, most notably, Rolf Kötter, for making the database publicly available. The research reported in this paper was sponsored by the Defense
Advanced Research Projects Agency, Defense Sciences Office, Program: Systems of Neuromorphic Adaptive
Plastic Scalable Electronics, under Contract HR0011-09-C-0002.

1

To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: dmodha@us.ibm.com (mailto:dmodha@us.ibm.com).

Author contributions: D.S.M. and R.S. designed research, performed research, analyzed data, and wrote the paper.
*It is important to draw a distinction between the actual physical network in a macaque brain and its logical description in network-theoretical terminology using reported data. Because we are primarily concerned with the latter usage in this paper, we will refer to network description as network.


CoCoMac also reports 13,498 plausible connections that were tested for but were not found. This substantially reduces the possibility that projections present in the brain are dramatically undersampled or underreported.


This usage of physical hierarchical partition of brain into its constituent parts is different from logical hierarchical information processing in visual cortex, as discussed in the article by Felleman and Van Essen (2).

§

Establishing homology between human and macaque cortical organization remains an ongoing and active research area
(23, 44–46), and it has been clearly noted that “homology cannot be proven but must be ‘inferred’” (47). Nonetheless, building on the conclusion in the article by Orban et al. (47) that “Despite several functional differences, many areas are homologous, especially at early levels of the visual hierarchy. In higher-order cortex, ‘regional’ homology still largely applies” and emboldened by the early functional MRI studies in mapping task-positive and task-negative networks in macaque (48), here, we assume that homology indeed holds.
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1008054107
/-/DCSupplemental (/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1008054107/-/DCSupplemental).
Freely available online through the PNAS open access option.

1. Steno N (1669) Dissertatio de cerebri anatome, spectatissimis viris dd Societatis apud dominum
Thevenot collectae, dictata, atque é gallico exemplari (Latinitate donata, opera and studio Guidonis
Fanosii, Paris).

12/5/2013 10:50 PM

Network architecture of the long-distance pathways in the macaque brain

http://www.pnas.org/content/107/30/13485.full

Search Google Scholar (http://scholar.google.com/scholar?as_q=& as_epq=Dissertatio%20de%20cerebri%20anatome %2C%20spectatissimis%20viris%20dd%20Societatis%20apud%20dominum%20Thevenot%20collectae
%2C%20dictata%2C%20atque%20%C3%83%C2%A9%20gallico%20exemplari&as_oq=&as_eq=&
Top as_occt=any&as_sauthors=Steno&as_publication=&as_ylo=&as_yhi=&btnG=&hl=en&sciui=1& Abstract as_sdt=0%2C5)

2. Felleman DJ, Van Essen DC (1991) Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex.

Model: Deriving the Network
Cereb Cortex 1:1–47.
Description
Abstract/FREE Full Text (/cgi/ijlink?linkType=ABST&journalCode=cercor&resid=1/1/1-a)

3.
ResultsUngerleider LG, Mishkin M (1982) in Analysis of Visual Behavior , eds Ingle DJ, Goodale MA, Mansfield
RJ (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA), pp 549–586.
Search Google Scholar (http://scholar.google.com/scholar?as_q=&
Discussion
as_epq=Analysis%20of%20Visual%20Behavior&as_oq=&as_eq=&as_occt=any&as_sauthors=Ingle& as_publication=&as_ylo=&as_yhi=&btnG=&hl=en&sciui=1&as_sdt=0%2C5) Acknowledgments

4. Schmahmann JD, Pandya DN (2006) Fiber Pathways of the Brain (Oxford Univ Press, New York).

FootnotesSearch Google Scholar (http://scholar.google.com/scholar?as_q=& as_epq=Fiber%20Pathways%20of%20the%20Brain&as_oq=&as_eq=&as_occt=any& References as_sauthors=Schmahmann&as_publication=&as_ylo=&as_yhi=&btnG=&hl=en&sciui=1&as_sdt=0%2C5) 8 of 11

5. Young MP (1993) The organization of neural systems in the primate cerebral cortex. Proc Biol Sci
252:13–18. Abstract/FREE Full Text (/cgi/ijlink?linkType=ABST&journalCode=royprsb&resid=252/1333/13)
6. Kaiser M, Hilgetag CC (2006) Nonoptimal component placement, but short processing paths, due to long-distance projections in neural systems. PLOS Comput Biol 2:e95.
CrossRef (/external-ref?access_num=10.1371/journal.pcbi.0020095&link_type=DOI)
Medline (/external-ref?access_num=16848638&link_type=MED)

7. Scannell JW, Burns GA, Hilgetag CC, O 'Neil MA, Young MP (1999) The connectional organization of the cortico-thalamic system of the cat. Cereb Cortex 9:277–299.
Abstract/FREE Full Text (/cgi/ijlink?linkType=ABST&journalCode=cercor&resid=9/3/277)

8. Young MP (1992) Objective analysis of the topological organization of the primate cortical visual system. Nature 358:152–155. CrossRef (/external-ref?access_num=10.1038/358152a0&link_type=DOI)
Medline (/external-ref?access_num=1614547&link_type=MED)

9. Hilgetag CC, O 'Neill MA, Young MP (1996) Indeterminate organization of the visual system. Science
271:776–777. FREE Full Text (/cgi/ijlink?linkType=FULL&journalCode=sci&resid=271/5250/776)
10. Stephan KE, et al. (2000) Computational analysis of functional connectivity between areas of primate cerebral cortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 355:111–126.
Abstract/FREE Full Text (/cgi/ijlink?linkType=ABST&journalCode=royptb&resid=355/1393/111)

11. Sporns O, Zwi JD (2004) The small world of the cerebral cortex. Neuroinformatics 2:145–162.
CrossRef (/external-ref?access_num=10.1385/NI:2:2:145&link_type=DOI)
Medline (/external-ref?access_num=15319512&link_type=MED)
Web of Science (/external-ref?access_num=000223582200003&link_type=ISI)

12. Sporns O, Kötter R (2004) Motifs in brain networks. PLoS Biol 2:e369.
CrossRef (/external-ref?access_num=10.1371/journal.pbio.0020369&link_type=DOI)
Medline (/external-ref?access_num=15510229&link_type=MED)

13. Sporns O, Honey CJ, Kötter R (2007) Identification and classification of hubs in brain networks. PLoS
ONE 2:e1049. CrossRef (/external-ref?access_num=10.1371/journal.pone.0001049&link_type=DOI)
Medline (/external-ref?access_num=17940613&link_type=MED)

14. Bohland JW, et al. (2009) A proposal for a coordinated effort for the determination of brainwide neuroanatomical connectivity in model organisms at a mesoscopic scale. PLOS Comput Biol
5:e1000334. CrossRef (/external-ref?access_num=10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000334&link_type=DOI)
Medline (/external-ref?access_num=19325892&link_type=MED)

15. Faloutsos M, Faloutsos P, Faloutsos C (1999) Proceedings of SIGCOMM '99 , On power-law relationships of the Internet topology (Association for Computing Machinery, New York), pp 251–262.
Search Google Scholar (http://scholar.google.com/scholar?as_q=&as_epq=On%20powerlaw%20relationships%20of%20the%20Internet%20topology&as_oq=&as_eq=&as_occt=any& as_sauthors=Faloutsos&as_publication=&as_ylo=&as_yhi=&btnG=&hl=en&sciui=1&as_sdt=0%2C5) 16. Newman MEJ, Barabási AL, Watts DJ (2006) The Structure and Dynamics of Networks (Princeton Univ
Press, Princeton).
Search Google Scholar (http://scholar.google.com/scholar?as_q=& as_epq=The%20Structure%20and%20Dynamics%20of%20Networks&as_oq=&as_eq=&as_occt=any& as_sauthors=Newman&as_publication=&as_ylo=&as_yhi=&btnG=&hl=en&sciui=1&as_sdt=0%2C5)

17. Seidman SB (1983) Network structure and minimum degree. Soc Networks 5:269–287.
CrossRef (/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/0378-8733(83)90028-X&link_type=DOI)
Web of Science (/external-ref?access_num=A1983RS06200002&link_type=ISI)

18. Brin S, Page L (1998) The anatomy of a large-scale hypertextual web search engine. Comput Netw

12/5/2013 10:50 PM

Network architecture of the long-distance pathways in the macaque brain

http://www.pnas.org/content/107/30/13485.full

ISDN Syst 30:107–117.
CrossRef (/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0169-7552(98)00110-X&link_type=DOI)

19. Kleinberg JM (1999) Authoritative sources in a hyperlinked environment. J ACM 46:604–632.
Top
CrossRef (/external-ref?access_num=10.1145/324133.324140&link_type=DOI)
Abstract

20. Stephan KE, Zilles K, Kötter R (2000) Coordinate-independent mapping of structural and functional data by objective relational transformation (ORT) Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 355:37–54.

Model: Deriving the Network
Abstract/FREE Full Text (/cgi/ijlink?linkType=ABST&journalCode=royptb&resid=355/1393/37)
Description

21. Stephan KE, et al. (2001) Advanced database methodology for the Collation of Connectivity data on the
ResultsMacaque brain (CoCoMac) Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 356:1159–1186.

Abstract/FREE Full Text (/cgi/ijlink?linkType=ABST&journalCode=royptb&resid=356/1412/1159)
Discussion

22. Kötter R (2004) Online retrieval, processing, and visualization of primate connectivity data from the

CoCoMac
Acknowledgments database. Neuroinformatics 2:127–144.
CrossRef (/external-ref?access_num=10.1385/NI:2:2:127&link_type=DOI)
FootnotesMedline (/external-ref?access_num=15319511&link_type=MED)
Web of Science (/external-ref?access_num=000223582200002&link_type=ISI)
References

9 of 11

23. Van Essen DC (2004) in The Visual Neurosciences, eds Chalupa L, Werner J (MIT Press, Cambridge,
MA), pp 507–521.
Search Google Scholar (http://scholar.google.com/scholar?as_q=& as_epq=The%20Visual%20Neurosciences&as_oq=&as_eq=&as_occt=any&as_sauthors=Chalupa& as_publication=&as_ylo=&as_yhi=&btnG=&hl=en&sciui=1&as_sdt=0%2C5)

24. Kötter R, Wanke E (2005) Mapping brains without coordinates. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci
360:751–766. CrossRef (/external-ref?access_num=10.1098/rstb.2005.1625&link_type=DOI)
Medline (/external-ref?access_num=15971361&link_type=MED)

25. Holten D (2006) Hierarchical edge bundles: Visualization of adjacency relations in hierarchical data.
IEEE Trans Vis Comput Graph 12:741–748.
CrossRef (/external-ref?access_num=10.1109/TVCG.2006.147&link_type=DOI)
Medline (/external-ref?access_num=17080795&link_type=MED)
Web of Science (/external-ref?access_num=000241383300012&link_type=ISI)

26. Sherman SM, Guillery RW (2006) Exploring the Thalamus and Its Role in Cortical Function (MIT Press,
Cambridge, MA).
Search Google Scholar (http://scholar.google.com/scholar?as_q=& as_epq=Exploring%20the%20Thalamus%20and%20Its%20Role%20in%20Cortical%20Function&as_oq=& as_eq=&as_occt=any&as_sauthors=Sherman&as_publication=&as_ylo=&as_yhi=&btnG=&hl=en&sciui=1& as_sdt=0%2C5) 27. Watts DJ, Strogatz SH (1998) Collective dynamics of ‘small-world’ networks. Nature 393:440–442.
CrossRef (/external-ref?access_num=10.1038/30918&link_type=DOI)
Medline (/external-ref?access_num=9623998&link_type=MED)

28. Milgram S (1967) Small-world problem. Psychol Today 1:61–67.
Web of Science (/external-ref?access_num=A1967ZK28400008&link_type=ISI)

29. Barabási AL, Bonabeau E (2003) Scale-free networks. Sci Am 288:60–69.
Medline (/external-ref?access_num=12701331&link_type=MED)

30. Erdös P, Rényi A (1960) On the evolution of random graphs. A Publ Math Inst Hung Acad Sci 5:17–61.
Search Google Scholar (http://scholar.google.com/scholar?as_q=& as_epq=On%20the%20evolution%20of%20random%20graphs&as_oq=&as_eq=&as_occt=any& as_sauthors=Erd%C3%83%C2%B6s&as_publication=&as_ylo=&as_yhi=&btnG=&hl=en&sciui=1& as_sdt=0%2C5) 31. Amaral LA, Scala A, Barthelemy M, Stanley HE (2000) Classes of small-world networks. Proc Natl
Acad Sci USA 97:11149–11152.
Abstract/FREE Full Text (/cgi/ijlink?linkType=ABST&journalCode=pnas&resid=97/21/11149)

32. Humphries MD, Gurney K, Prescott TJ (2006) The brainstem reticular formation is a small-world, not scale-free, network. Proc Biol Sci 273:503–511.
Abstract/FREE Full Text (/cgi/ijlink?linkType=ABST&journalCode=royprsb&resid=273/1585/503)

33. Eguíluz VM, Chialvo DR, Cecchi GA, Baliki M, Apkarian AV (2005) Scale-free brain functional networks.
Phys Rev Lett 94:018102.
CrossRef (/external-ref?access_num=10.1103/PhysRevLett.94.018102&link_type=DOI)
Medline (/external-ref?access_num=15698136&link_type=MED)

34. Achard S, Salvador R, Whitcher B, Suckling J, Bullmore E (2006) A resilient, low-frequency, small-world human brain functional network with highly connected association cortical hubs. J Neurosci 26:63–72.
Abstract/FREE Full Text (/cgi/ijlink?linkType=ABST&journalCode=jneuro&resid=26/1/63)

35. Kaiser M, Martin R, Andras P, Young MP (2007) Simulation of robustness against lesions of cortical networks. Eur J Neurosci 25:3185–3192.
CrossRef (/external-ref?access_num=10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05574.x&link_type=DOI)

12/5/2013 10:50 PM

Network architecture of the long-distance pathways in the macaque brain

http://www.pnas.org/content/107/30/13485.full

Medline (/external-ref?access_num=17561832&link_type=MED)
Web of Science (/external-ref?access_num=000247115300027&link_type=ISI)

36. Albert R, Jeong H, Barabási AL (2000) Error and attack tolerance of complex networks. Nature
Top
406:378–382. CrossRef (/external-ref?access_num=10.1038/35019019&link_type=DOI)
Abstract Medline (/external-ref?access_num=10935628&link_type=MED)

37. Clauset A, Shalizi C, Newman MEJ (2009) Power-law distributions in empirical data. SIAM Rev

Model: Deriving the Network
51:661–703. CrossRef (/external-ref?access_num=10.1137/070710111&link_type=DOI)
Description

38. White JG, Southgate E, Thomson JN, Brenner S (1996) The structure of the nervous system of the
Resultsnematode Caenorhabditis elegans. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 314:1–340.
Search Google Scholar (http://scholar.google.com/scholar?as_q=&
Discussion
as_epq=The%20structure%20of%20the%20nervous%20system%20of%20the%20nematode%20Caenorhabditis%20elegans& as_oq=&as_eq=&as_occt=any&as_sauthors=White&as_publication=&as_ylo=&as_yhi=&btnG=&hl=en&sciui=1& as_sdt=0%2C5)
Acknowledgments

39. Keller EF (2005) Revisiting “scale-free” networks. Bioessays 27:1060–1068.

Footnotes
CrossRef (/external-ref?access_num=10.1002/bies.20294&link_type=DOI)

Medline (/external-ref?access_num=16163729&link_type=MED)
References
Web of Science (/external-ref?access_num=000232361100009&link_type=ISI)

40. Newman MEJ (2005) A measure of betweenness centrality based on random walks. Soc Networks
27:39–54. CrossRef (/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.socnet.2004.11.009&link_type=DOI)
Web of Science (/external-ref?access_num=000227500500003&link_type=ISI)

41. Freeman LC (1977) A set of measures of centrality based on betweenness. Sociometry 40:35–41.
CrossRef (/external-ref?access_num=10.2307/3033543&link_type=DOI)
Web of Science (/external-ref?access_num=A1977CZ20900004&link_type=ISI)

42. de Nooy W, Mrvar A, Batagelj V (2005) Exploratory Social Network Analysis with Pajek (Cambridge
Univ Press, New York).
Search Google Scholar (http://scholar.google.com/scholar?as_q=& as_epq=Exploratory%20Social%20Network%20Analysis%20with%20Pajek&as_oq=&as_eq=&as_occt=any& as_sauthors=de%20Nooy&as_publication=&as_ylo=&as_yhi=&btnG=&hl=en&sciui=1&as_sdt=0%2C5)

43. Fox MD, et al. (2005) The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102:9673–9678.
Abstract/FREE Full Text (/cgi/ijlink?linkType=ABST&journalCode=pnas&resid=102/27/9673)

44. Orban GA, et al. (2003) Similarities and differences in motion processing between the human and macaque brain: evidence from fMRI. Neuropsychologia 41:1757–1768.
CrossRef (/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0028-3932(03)00177-5&link_type=DOI)
Medline (/external-ref?access_num=14527539&link_type=MED)
Web of Science (/external-ref?access_num=000186215700005&link_type=ISI)

45. Astafiev SV, et al. (2003) Functional organization of human intraparietal and frontal cortex for attending, looking, and pointing. J Neurosci 23:4689–4699.
Abstract/FREE Full Text (/cgi/ijlink?linkType=ABST&journalCode=jneuro&resid=23/11/4689)

46. Orban GA, Van Essen DC, Vanduffel W (2004) Comparative mapping of higher visual areas in monkeys and humans. Trends Cogn Sci 8:315–324.
CrossRef (/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.tics.2004.05.009&link_type=DOI)
Medline (/external-ref?access_num=15242691&link_type=MED)
Web of Science (/external-ref?access_num=000222862500009&link_type=ISI)

47. Orban GA, et al. (2006) Mapping the parietal cortex of human and non-human primates.
Neuropsychologia 44:2647–2667.
CrossRef (/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.11.001&link_type=DOI)
Medline (/external-ref?access_num=16343560&link_type=MED)

48. Vincent JL, et al. (2007) Intrinsic functional architecture in the anaesthetized monkey brain. Nature
447:83–86. CrossRef (/external-ref?access_num=10.1038/nature05758&link_type=DOI)

10 of 11

Medline (/external-ref?access_num=17476267&link_type=MED)

Cortical High-Density Counterstream Architectures
Science (Science) 2013 342 (6158) 1238406
Abstract (http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/abstract/342/6158/1238406)
(http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/342/6158/1238406)

Full Text (HTML)

Full Text (PDF) (http://www.sciencemag.org

/cgi/reprint/342/6158/1238406)

Systematic, Cross-Cortex Variation in Neuron Numbers in Rodents and Primates
Cereb Cortex (Cerebral Cortex) 2013 0 (2013) bht214v1-bht214

12/5/2013 10:50 PM

Network architecture of the long-distance pathways in the macaque brain
Abstract (http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/content/abstract/bht214v1)
(http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/content/full/bht214v1)
Top

http://www.pnas.org/content/107/30/13485.full

Full Text (HTML)

Full Text (PDF) (http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/reprint

/bht214v1)

Abstract

Mapping the Hierarchical Layout of the Structural Network of the Macaque Prefrontal Cortex

Cereb Cortex Network
Model: Deriving the(Cerebral Cortex) 2012 0 (2012) bhs399v1-bhs399
Abstract (http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/content/abstract/bhs399v1)
Full Text (HTML)
Description
Full Text (PDF) (http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/reprint
(http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/content/full/bhs399v1)
Results
/bhs399v1)
Discussion

The brain 's connective core and its role in animal cognition
AcknowledgmentsSoc B (Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences) 2012 367
Phil Trans R

(1603) 2704-2714

Footnotes
Abstract (http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/cgi/content/abstract/367/1603/2704)

11 of 11

Full Text (HTML)

Full Text (PDF)

(http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/cgi/content/full/367/1603/2704)
References
(http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/cgi/reprint/367/1603/2704)

Resting-State Connectivity Identifies Distinct Functional Networks in Macaque Cingulate Cortex
Cereb Cortex (Cerebral Cortex) 2012 22 (6) 1294-1308
Abstract (http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/content/abstract/22/6/1294)
(http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/content/full/22/6/1294)

Full Text (HTML)

Full Text (PDF) (http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/reprint

/22/6/1294)

Hemispheric Asymmetry and Visuo-Olfactory Integration in Perceiving Subthreshold (Micro)
Fearful Expressions
J. Neurosci. (Journal of Neuroscience) 2012 32 (6) 2159-2165
Abstract (http://www.jneurosci.org/cgi/content/abstract/32/6/2159)
/cgi/content/full/32/6/2159)

Full Text (HTML) (http://www.jneurosci.org

Full Text (PDF) (http://www.jneurosci.org/cgi/reprint/32/6/2159)

Gerstmann Meets Geschwind: A Crossing (or Kissing) Variant of a Subcortical Disconnection
Syndrome?
Neuroscientist (The Neuroscientist) 2011 17 (6) 633-644
Abstract (http://nro.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/17/6/633)

Full Text (PDF) (http://nro.sagepub.com/cgi/reprint

/17/6/633)

12/5/2013 10:50 PM

References: (http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/cgi/reprint/367/1603/2704) Resting-State Connectivity Identifies Distinct Functional Networks in Macaque Cingulate Cortex Cereb Cortex (Cerebral Cortex) 2012 22 (6) 1294-1308 Abstract (http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/content/abstract/22/6/1294) (http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/content/full/22/6/1294) Full Text (HTML) Full Text (PDF) (http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/reprint /22/6/1294) J. Neurosci. (Journal of Neuroscience) 2012 32 (6) 2159-2165 Abstract (http://www.jneurosci.org/cgi/content/abstract/32/6/2159) /cgi/content/full/32/6/2159) Full Text (HTML) (http://www.jneurosci.org Full Text (PDF) (http://www.jneurosci.org/cgi/reprint/32/6/2159) Gerstmann Meets Geschwind: A Crossing (or Kissing) Variant of a Subcortical Disconnection

You May Also Find These Documents Helpful

  • Good Essays

    The body is mapped onto the motor complex by the networks of the brain in an organized and systematic way, with certain parts of the motor and the somatosensory cortex mapping onto certain parts of the body. (pg.102)…

    • 1295 Words
    • 5 Pages
    Good Essays
  • Satisfactory Essays

    Alva Noe

    • 465 Words
    • 2 Pages

    The brain is a very important part of our body.It is the reason we are as inteligent as we are now.The Brain is basically our main body part.It controls our whole body and sends responses so we can move.Sabastian Seung is making a map to show the brain and Noe argues that the connectome is useless because its not advancing brain research and its just a useless map.Alva Noe convinces us that the brain map wont be useful by providing evidence and using different authors crafts to provide a stronger argument.…

    • 465 Words
    • 2 Pages
    Satisfactory Essays
  • Satisfactory Essays

    Neuro Study Guide

    • 702 Words
    • 3 Pages

    Cerebral Cortex * Frontal Lobes * Primary motor cortex – voluntary movement * Association cortex – sequencing; planning and social skills, representational memory, impulse control, language * Parietal Lobes * Primary somatosensory cortex – sensory info * Association cortex – body awareness (L), space awareness (R) * Facial recognition * Temporal Lobes * Primary auditory cortex – hearing * Association cortex – meaning * Occipital Lobes * Primary visual cortex – simple features * Association cortex – complex integration DIVISION OF THE BRAIN Central Hempishere ___________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________ Contralateral Control ___________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________ Cerebral Cortex ____________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________ DIVISION OF THE BRAIN Primary Sensory & Motor Cortex * Space is devoted according to importance, neural density and complexity of the region * Contralateral connections with sensory organs and muscles Speech Centres * Damage to left auditory association cortex = language comprehension deficits *…

    • 702 Words
    • 3 Pages
    Satisfactory Essays
  • Powerful Essays

    Gould, E., A. J. Reeves, M. S. A. Graziano and C. G. Gross. 1999. Neurogenesis in the neocortex of adult primates. Science 286: 54-552.…

    • 1866 Words
    • 8 Pages
    Powerful Essays
  • Good Essays

    The human brain is marked not by overall size but by advanced corticalization, or enragement of the cerebral cortex.…

    • 596 Words
    • 3 Pages
    Good Essays
  • Good Essays

    The human brain is a complex and sophisticated organ. Understanding the function of the brain is often limited to the understanding of the brains areas with regard to how these areas respond to stimuli or in cases of damage. Much of the understanding of the brain is rooted in observation of damaged brains and their correlation of impaired function with specific areas of damage. Modern technologies have begun to change this trend because tools such as the Magnetic Resonance Imager (MRI) allows scientist to observe brain function with the invasiveness of surgery. This technology has provided not just insights into neuroscience but also into psychology as brain functions can now be correlated better with behavior and heredity. One can see this insight when examining specific areas of the brain such as the temporal and frontal lobes of the brain.…

    • 767 Words
    • 3 Pages
    Good Essays
  • Powerful Essays

    Chemical Dependency

    • 2244 Words
    • 9 Pages

    References: American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders(DSM-IV-TR) (4th Edition ed.). 1000 Wilson Blvd, Arlington, VA 22209-3901: American Psychiatric Publishing…

    • 2244 Words
    • 9 Pages
    Powerful Essays
  • Better Essays

    Homosexuality and Religion

    • 2244 Words
    • 9 Pages

    Cited: American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd ed., Revised). Washington, DC: Author.…

    • 2244 Words
    • 9 Pages
    Better Essays
  • Good Essays

    Human Primate Brains

    • 1694 Words
    • 7 Pages

    According to Rilling (2014), understanding the evolution of the unique characteristics of the human brain requires studying the brain of other living primate species. In other words, a specific evolutionary change in the human brain cannot be inferred to be unique to the human lineage unless other species sharing a last common ancestor don’t have it. That being said, Rilling emphasizes the role of comparative neuroimaging to investigate the similarities and differences between human and non-human primate brains, and highlights the different imaging techniques that have been used in multiple studies including structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), positron emission tomography (PET), functional MRI and diffusion-weighted imaging (DWI).…

    • 1694 Words
    • 7 Pages
    Good Essays
  • Good Essays

    support for the social brain theory. However, differences between taxonomic orders in the stability of the transition between…

    • 2832 Words
    • 12 Pages
    Good Essays
  • Powerful Essays

    Mental Disorder Paper

    • 1793 Words
    • 8 Pages

    |“The National Council for Behavioral Healthcare.” National Mental Health Association. N.p., n.d. Web. 06 Mar. 2013. |…

    • 1793 Words
    • 8 Pages
    Powerful Essays
  • Better Essays

    Central Idea: Advances in neuroscience have changed our understanding of the brain over time and created endless possibilities for the future.…

    • 1156 Words
    • 5 Pages
    Better Essays
  • Good Essays

    The mammalian brain is the most complex and the largest out of all the vertebrates. It has special features and characteristics that the others do not. “The unimpressive appearance of the human brain gives few hints of its remarkable abilities. It is about two…

    • 2636 Words
    • 8 Pages
    Good Essays
  • Powerful Essays

    artificial Neural Networks

    • 6762 Words
    • 28 Pages

    the 1970s. Moreover, it is truly remarkable to find that the perceptron (in its basic form…

    • 6762 Words
    • 28 Pages
    Powerful Essays