REVIEW OF LITERATURE
This chapter comprises six main sections that summarize the theoretical and empirical knowledge base regarding second language development and academic achievement in a second language. At the end of each main section,4 there is a summary synthesizing studies and highlighting key findings relevant to the present study. The first section reviews selected second language acquisition theories that reflect representative, current trends in the field and provide a theoretical foundation for the study. The second section focuses on defining language proficiency and reviews relevant studies illuminating linguistic factors implicated in ELLs' schooling. The third section summarizes the language learning strategy research base. The fourth section identifies relevant theories and research regarding motivation for learning a second language and makes connections among motivation, language learning strategies, and proficiency. The fifth section reviews studies that have examined academic achievement in second language. The last section summarizes the present study's variables as generated from this review of the literature.
Second Language Acquisition
Defining the Field
Second language acquisition (SLA) is an interdisciplinary field that is both historically old and new (Gass & Seliker, 2008). The field is old because the nature of
4 Except the first section which integrates theory and empirical findings throughout.
16
second language learning and teaching has fascinated scholars for centuries. In more modem terms, SLA is a young discipline which, beginning in the 1960s, distinguished itself from applied linguistics and education (Long, 2006). SLA focuses on second language (L2), as well as second dialect, learning and loss by children and adults. As formulated by Saville-Troike (2006), SLA seeks to answer three main questions: ( a)
"What exactly does the L2 learner know?" (b) "How does the learner acquire this knowledge?" and (c) "Why are some learners more successful than others?" (p. 24). From its beginnings, SLA has taken a multidisciplinary approach and drawn on other established disciplines including education, linguistics, psychology, anthropology, and sociology (Gass & Seliker, 2008).
The theoretical rationales in considering the connections between SLA and academic achievement research are twofold. First, as discussed in Chapter 1, a current focus in educational literature is on linguistic factors implicated in ELLs' schooling. In fact, many scholars find it difficult to distinguish between language proficiency and academic competence (Abedi & Lord, 2001; Solorzano, 2008); some scholars speak in terms of academic English development (SchleppegreU, 2004) or of academic achievement in second language (Collier, 1987; Collier & Thomas, 1989). Second, language learning strategies, the primary variable of interest to this study, have been chiefly examined from the SLA perspective.
Selected SLA Theoretical Frameworks
Language as a faculty of the mind. Early research on second language (L2) development was strongly influenced by research on first language (L 1) acquisition and by nativist linguistic theories. Nativist theories posit that language acquisition is
17
accomplished through the use of innate linguistic abilities. Many nativists assert that L 1 and L2 acquisition are similar in that they rely on essentially the same processes.
Chomsky (1959) hypothesized that language learners construct a theory of grammar from linguistic input using a "built-in," genetically endowed hypothesisforming device, also known as the language faculty or the language acquisition device
(LAD). Contrary to then-popular behaviorist ideas (Skinner, 1957), Chomsky argued that language acquisition cannot be solely attributed to learning through stimulus-responsereinforcement mechanisms. First, he pointed out that proficient speakers of a language can produce and comprehend novel, unheard-of sentences. Rather than a learned repertoire of prefabricated responses, as proposed by behaviorists, this creative ability suggests the existence of a special program, or language faculty, that assists humans in generating "an unlimited set of sentences out ofa finite list of words" (Pinker, 2007, p.
9).
Second, Chomsky noted the fact that all children, largely independently of intelligence, construct grammar in comparable ways within a remarkably short time and often without explicit reinforcement on the part of their parents (as in the case of young
English language learners). Chomsky proposed that these two phenomena could be explained by speakers' individual contributions such as "inborn structure, the genetically determined course of maturation and past experience" (p. 27). Pinker (2007) termed this inborn structure or program language instinct and argued that language was "a biological adaptation to communicative information" (p. 5). Chomskian ideas received a large following among linguists and psychologists; as a consequence, language acquisition came to be understood as a gradual, largely unconscious process of syntactic rules
18
deduction and subsequent acquisition. Rules deduction was thought to be triggered by linguistic input and assisted by LAD.
Nativist views on L2 acquisition are exemplified by Krashen's theory ofSLA.
Krashen (1985, 1987) argued that an L2 was acquired, for the most part, unconsciously and in the presence of what he termed comprehensible input (i.e., input that contains linguistic features slightly beyond the leamer's current level of proficiency). One of the key hypotheses of the theory, the Acquisition/Learning hypothesis, distinguished between language learning and language acquisition. Language acquisition was said to be an unconscious process of rules extraction equally accessible to children and adults.
Language learning, on the other hand, was defined as the conscious learning of rules through formal instruction and error correction.
An additional component ofKrashen's theory, the Affective Filter hypothesis, was proposed to account for individual differences in language acquisition. According to this hypothesis, affective factors (e.g., anxiety, motivation, or self-confidence) influence, on subconscious level, how much information L2 learners extract from available input.
Krashen (1985) argued that the Affective Filter hypothesis was supported by research documenting faster development in "lower filter" versus "higher filter" L2 learners exposed to the same amount ofL2 input. In sum, according to Krashen's SLA theory, people acquire an L2 when two conditions are met: (a) input is made available and comprehensible for language learners, and (b) learner Affective Filter is low to allow the intake of the input.
While Krashen's SLA theory attracted many followers and had a tremendous impact on L2 pedagogy in the 1980s (Spada, 1997), classroom research has provided
19
counterevidence to Krashen's hypothesis that speaking would naturally "emerge" in the presence of comprehensible input. Studies have shown that in input-based instructional programs (such as L2 immersion), L2 learners achieved native-like levels in comprehension (reading and oral comprehension), but not in production (writing and speaking; Swain, 2005). Moreover, contrary to Krashen's predictions, explicit instruction was found to benefit language learning. In her review of over 40 descriptive, quasiexperimental, and laboratory studies conducted with school-aged and adult L2 learners,
Spada concluded that, when combined with a focus on meaning, form-focused instruction
(i.e., instruction that deliberately focuses learners' attention on L2 grammatical features) does make a positive difference in L2 acquisition.
Moreover, many researchers found Krashen's SLA theory to be inadequate when it came to explaining a much greater variability in older L2 learners (Gregg, 1984;
Robinson, 1995; Schmidt, 1990). Gregg, for example, argued that older learners relied on their superior cognitive (not language-specific) abilities oflogic and problem-solving in order to "construct" L2 grammar rules. Additionally, the fact that only in rare cases did older learners achieve native-like proficiency (Bialystok & Hakuta, 1999; Hakuta,
Bialystok, & Wiley, 2003; Long, 1990) led some SLA researchers to conclude that Ll and L2 acquisition differed in some fundamental ways. Newport (1990), for example, proposed that older learners' superior cognitive abilities of memory and perception
(although associated with acquisition of larger chucks of language) prohibited componential analysis available to young children, thus accounting for the child-adult discrepancies in ultimate L2 attainment.
20
Several authors observed that the role of consciousness in L2 learning was considerably greater than suggested by some Ll theories (Robinson, 1995; Schmidt,
1990). O'Malley, Chamot, and Walker (1987), for example, argued that cognitive learning theories, discussed next, may provide a more appropriate framework for understanding and studying processes involved in L2 acquisition.
Language as a cognitive skill. Whereas nativist linguistic theories hold that language is a genetically endowed faculty of the mind, learning theories (Anderson,
1982, 1989; Skehan, 1998) posit that L2 is a complex cognitive skill whose acquisition is comparable to the learning of other complex skills such as problem solving. That is, linguistic codes are thought to be acquired, stored, and retrieved from memory much in the same way as any other information (O'Malley et aI., 1987). Learning is believed to be the result of language processing itself, where linguistic knowledge ("knowledge that") is transformed into linguistic performance ("knowledge how") through rules extraction, learning, and automatization (Long, 2006; Saville-Troike, 2006). Cognitive learning theories thus study how new information, including new L2 information, is processed and stored in memory. One of the cognitive theories that informed SLA research, and in particular LLS research, has been Anderson's Adoptive Control of Thought (ACT)
Theory (Chamot, 2005b; O'Malley et aI., 1987).
Anderson (1982, 1989) distinguished between two types of knowledge (or memory): (a) declarative knowledge, information stored in the form of facts; and (b) procedural knowledge, production rules mentally represented in the form of IF ITHEN5
(condition-action) pairs. This declarative-procedural knowledge distinction roughly
5 One example of an IF ITHEN pair is, "IF the goal is to generate the present tense of HUG, THEN say
HUG + s" (Anderson, 1989, p. 325).
21
corresponds to what Chomsky termed the distinction between linguistic competence (i.e., mentally represented knowledge of grammar) and linguistic performance (i.e., the use of linguistic knowledge in language comprehension and production; as cited in Fromkin,
Rodman, & Hyams, 2007).
According to Anderson's (1989) ACT model of information flow, the interplay between declarative and procedural knowledge, which takes place in working memory, results in the learning of complex cognitive skills. Figure I graphically represents the flow of information within the ACT framework.
DECLARATIVE
MEMORY
PROCEDURAL
MEMORY
RETRIEVE MATCH &
EXECUTE
STORE
ENCODE
WORKING
MEMORY
ACT
Figure 1.1. The flow of information within the ACT framework (Anderson, 1989, p.
319).
22
Anderson (1982) proposed three stages6 of the skill acquisition process: (a) declarative, (b) knowledge compilation, and (c) procedural. In the declarative stage of skill acquisition, the learner consciously extracts, or receives through instruction, factual information (e.g., patterns or rules) about a skill. This declarative knowledge is rehearsed and interpreted in short-term memory to generate "primitive rules" (productions) that specify the type of actions to be taken under a given set of circumstances. At this stage, a leamer's behavior is guided by a trial-and-error exploration in which the already existing productions (stored in long-term, declarative memory) are applied to new information.
This interpretive process is largely conscious and places a rather taxing demand on the leamer's processing capacities. O'Malley et al. (1987) argued that in terms ofL2 developmental stages, Anderson's declarative stage may explain the silent period-also known as delayed production-a period during which language learners do not engage in
L2 conversations. During this time, L2 learners are focused on developing a knowledge base about the new language. When first attempts at communication are made, beginning-level language learners with limited L2 declarative knowledge often recur to their L 1 linguistic forms (e.g., vocabulary, grammatical structures, and discourse patterns). The next stage, knowledge compilation, has been described as a "gradual process by which the knowledge is converted from declarative to procedural form" (Anderson,
1982, p. 370). This is realized by two processes: (a) collapsing simple production rules into single rules with an effect of a sequence (metaproductions); and (b) eliminating references to the declarative knowledge (proceduralization). That is, to perform a task,
6 In some sources, these three stages are referred to as cognition, association, and autonomy (McDonough,
1999).
23 the learner no longer needs to retrieve declarative knowledge into working memory. This stage results in a considerable speedup of the processing and ultimately allows the learner to complete a task in a single trial without verbal rehearsal in short-term memory.
According to O'Malley and colleagues (1987), this stage may correspond to the development of what linguists termed interlanguage (i.e., learner intermediate linguistic system between an Ll and an L2).
In the procedural stage, the learner becomes more efficient through experience
(Anderson, 1982, 1989). This process involves the gradual fine-tuning of metaproductions; that is, general rules transferrable to novel situations are extracted and the conditions under which these rules can be applied are specified. During this stage, the amount of practice and, by implication, the ability to generate opportunities for practice are key to successful skills development. This is because every time that factual or procedural information is "fired" (selected) for the performance of a particular task, knowledge is strengthened. In SLA, this stage is described as automatization (i.e., the development of effortless and largely unconscious skill performance; Skehan, 1998).
Automatization corresponds to fluency development, in which the performance of a language learner gradually approaches that ofa native speaker. O'Malley et al. (1987) argued that while declarative knowledge can be acquired relatively quickly, the development of procedural knowledge requires a prolonged period of time. The authors also noted that declarative knowledge is not a sufficient condition for L2 production.
An alternative explanation of processes involved in language learning was proposed by social theories. These theories "underscore the social nature" of both Ll and
24
L2 learning (Gersten & Hudelson, 2005, p. 23). The next section discusses one such theory, namely Vygotsky's (1978) sociocultural theory.
Language as a socially constructed skill. Unlike cognitive learning theories, social theories consider the quality of contact with new language (i.e., social interactions), and not the amount of L2 experience to be at the core of L2 acquisition
(Long, 2006). Children are believed to develop language and, importantly, cognition over time through multiple interactions with more capable others (adults and peers).
According to Vygotsky's (1978) sociocultural theory, the nature of social interactions defines the what (the content) and the how (how information is interpreted, organized, and retained) of the child's learning. Learning occurs through the transformation of simple mental activities (e.g., labeling) to higher mental activities (e.g., abstraction) by means of language, mediation, and internalization (Leong & Bodrova, 1995). Thus, the development of language and cognition are perceived as interdependent processes.
Vygotsky (1978) wrote, "the only 'good learning' is that which is in advance of development" (p. 89). Such "good learning" can only happen with expert assistance and within the Zone of Proximal Development (ZPD), which he defined as follows: "It is the distance between the actual developmental level as determined by independent problem solving and the level of potential development as determined through problem solving under adult guidance or in collaboration with more capable peers" (p. 86).
In early developmental stages, language is external to the learner. First, children use language to solve problems on an interpersonal level (within the ZPD) when they negotiate the task with more capable others (adults or more advanced peers). Children then internalize language to develop inner or intrapersonal speech: "Instead of appealing
25
to the adults, children appeal to themselves" (p. 27). Thus language becomes the planning device for organizing one's conceptual understandings with a sociocultural motivation to share with others.
As applied to L2 development, the concept of ZPD may be realized through what
SLA researchers term modified input (i.e., the use of short, low syntactic complexity utterances) or modified interaction (i.e., frequent comprehension checks, clarification requests, and self- and other-repetition; Long, 1983, 1987). Modified input and interaction are the means by which language experts provide language learners with ready-to-use chunks of speech, thus enabling L2 learners to express themselves beyond their current means (Saville-Troike, 2006). Among intrapersonal speech types observed in L2 learners, Saville-Troike distinguished: ( a) private speech, an audible talk to self, usually present in young children; (b) private writing, writing for oneself (e.g., lists of new words, translations into L 1, or notes on the margins of a book); and (c) inner speech, inside-the-head speech. Saville-Troike reported on studies documenting L2 learners using intrapersonal speech to actively build up their L2 competence even when they were not directly engaged in L2 communication.
In sum, from Vygotsky's (1978) sociocultural perspective, learning is realized through interpersonal and intrapersonal social mediation. This model predicts differentiated performance among L2learners depending on: (a) learners' access to and amount of participation in a learning community; (b) the amount of mediation from experts and peers; and (c) the degree to which learners use that help (Saville-Troike,
26
2006). Similarly to Anderson's (1982, 1989) theoretical work, Vygotsky's theory has been informative to LLS research.7
Summary
You May Also Find These Documents Helpful
-
on all four components of language acquisition: reading, writing, listening and speaking, as well as the culture of…
- 1100 Words
- 5 Pages
Powerful Essays -
Lantolf, J. P., & Thorne, S. L. (2006). Sociocultural theory and the genesis of second language…
- 452 Words
- 2 Pages
Satisfactory Essays -
The purpose of this task is to provide you with the opportunity to apply theoretical concepts…
- 1931 Words
- 17 Pages
Good Essays -
Lantoff. J., & Thorne. S. (2006). Sociocultural Theory and the Genesis of Second Language Development. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.…
- 56 Words
- 1 Page
Satisfactory Essays -
Klapper, J (1998) Language Learning at School and University , Language Learning journal 18, p23-27…
- 6976 Words
- 24 Pages
Powerful Essays -
In order to support my analysis, I will be mentioning some theoretical concepts that will be covered and…
- 2997 Words
- 12 Pages
Better Essays -
There is an old saying, “Everyday we should be learning something new, If your not your probably dead.” People are developing no matter our age, but it is how children develop and learn that sets each one of us apart. Child developments is at the earliest ages affects all aspects of a childs life. Throughout the class, we looked at many theorists during the course of the semester as well as looked at many articles pertaining to the concepts of the development of children. The theorists and articles opened up our minds to a world that we have never seen before and concepts about child development we have never been taught but have seen in the Lois Bloom and Margaret Lahey describe in their book Language Development and Language Disorders “language is like a code, it is a means of representation” Although, everyone is able to interpret the use of language some interpret it more than others. This delay in language, we have learned, could be for a variety of reasons. The way a child understands language will partially be based on the child’s background: how they were brought up, who they were brought up by, what kind of condition they were brought up in, what kind of parents the child was brought up by, if was the child brought up in poverty, etc. Language is a key part in anyone’s life as it is a major form of communication. Language is what separates us from other species to be intellectual and to rule society. Language development is important in a child’s life as it will pave the way for successful communication with various other children and other people in society. By means of talking to one another when we are young they stated, development of language skills offer a critical foundation for children’s school readiness and academic achievement.…
- 2450 Words
- 6 Pages
Better Essays -
PRELIMINARY LITERATURE REVIEW/THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK: Literature relevant to topic; main trends/debates/arguments; context of topic in previous research.…
- 5544 Words
- 23 Pages
Good Essays -
Teaching an L2 or a second language to students sometimes is very difficult and we ask ourselves how to teach the language. Many authors have tried to investigate the way to teach a language and how we acquire a second language or L2 with the influences of an L1 or our mother tongue.…
- 927 Words
- 4 Pages
Good Essays -
Second language acquisition is a process by which people learn a second language. It refers to any language learned in addition to the first language and the differences between both processes. Second language acquisition is a controversial issue; since, there is no a general agreement about how and when it takes place. Several theories and models have tried to explain the facts that may be involved in acquisition of a second language, Theories as the Behaviourism theories which state that acquisition is just a set of habits a learner may acquire or Nativist theories that suggest we all have a natural-born ability to acquire a language or models as Socio –linguistic models, Interactional models that express that language and society are connected and the former is affected by society , etc .However all the theories have failed to explain how acquisition really happen and yet there are many questions unsolved .…
- 2577 Words
- 11 Pages
Powerful Essays -
This is not an academic paper on theoretical discussions but rather a series of practical questions and…
- 1514 Words
- 8 Pages
Powerful Essays -
This Chapter presents the Relevant Theories, Related Literature and Studies, Conceptual Framework, Hypothesis and the Definition of Terms used in the study.…
- 2806 Words
- 12 Pages
Powerful Essays -
Part One, Background of the Study and Theoretical Framework of the Study, presents the rationale for choosing the problem and the Theoretical Framework upon which study was anchored.…
- 2158 Words
- 9 Pages
Powerful Essays -
For the purpose of this study, the work will be guided by the following hypotheses:…
- 3188 Words
- 11 Pages
Good Essays -
This chapter presents the background of the study, problem and its significance, and the scope and delimitation of the study.…
- 1272 Words
- 6 Pages
Powerful Essays