Intelligence can be described in many ways with many tests focusing on an individual’s cognitive abilities and failing to account for the social and practical aspects of intelligence. Tests to measure intelligence vary immensely and test different types of intelligence; such as Emotional Intelligence, which has proved popular in more recent years. Although researchers are unable to agree upon a general definition of intelligence they do agree that there are 2 factors to be included in a broad definition: an individual’s ability to adapt to their environment and a capacity to learn from experience (Sternberg and Detterman, 1986). Tests do not , however, measure these components very effectively thus intelligence is sometimes defined operationally, in terms of what a particular test measures. This essay will outline some theories and will critically evaluate their capacity for measuring intelligence.
The standard and most widely accepted method of measuring intelligence is through psychometric tests that measure a person’s Intelligence Quotient (IQ). A person’s IQ is determined from a series of tests that assess various types of abilities such as mathematical, spatial, verbal, logic and memory. The results from these tests obtained from a wide population show a classic ‘bell-shape’ distribution, meaning that most people are of average intelligence with a few at the extreme ends of the scale. For example, an individual who had an IQ lower than 50 would be seen to be mentally inadequate while someone with an IQ above 130 is seen as gifted or, further still, a genius, but more about this later.
In 1904, Spearman suggested that intelligence is comprised of a general factor (g), which is shared with all other mental abilities, and other specific factors (s), which are shared with none. After using a technique known as factor analysis to examine a number of mental aptitude tests, he concluded that
References: Ackerman, P. L., Beier, M. E. and Boyle, M. O. (2005). Working memory and intelligence: the same or different constructs?, Psychological Bulletin, 131, 30-60. Burns, N. R., Nettlebeck, T. and McPherson, J. (2009). Attention and intelligence: a factor analytic study, Journal of Individual Differences, 30, 44-57. Cattell, R. B. (1971). Abilities: Their Structure, Growth and Action, Boston, MA, Houghton Mifflin. Evans, J. J., Floyd, R. G., McGrew, K. S. and Leforgee, M. H. (2001). The relations between measures of Cattell-Horn-Carroll (CHC) cognitive abilities and reading achievement during childhood and adolescence, School Psychology Review, 31, 246-62. Floyd, R. G., Evans, J. J. and McGrew, K. S. (2003). Relations between measures of Cattell-Horn-Carroll (CHC) cognitive abilities and mathematics achievement across the school-age years, Psychology in Schools, 40, 155-71. Flynn, J. R. (1987). Massive IQ gains in 14 nations: What IQ tests really measure, Psychological Bulletin, 101, 171-191. Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of mind: The theory of multiple intelligences. New York: Basic Books. Goleman, D. (1995). Emotional intelligence: Why it can matter more than IQ. New York: Bantam. Kyllonen, P. C. (2002). g: Knowledge, speed, strategies, or working memory capacity? A systems perspective. In R. J. Sternberg, & E. L. Gigorenko (Eds.), The general factor of intelligence: How general is it? (pp. 415–445). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. McEnrue, M. P. and Groves, K. (2006). Choosing among tests of emotional intelligence: What is the evidence?, Human Resource Development Quarterly, 17(1), 9-42. McGrew, K. S. (2009). CHC theory and the human cognitive abilities project: Standing on the shoulders of the giants of psychometric intelligence research. Intelligence, 37(1), 1-10. Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. R. (2008b). What is emotional intelligence and what does it predict? In P. Kyllonen, R. Roberts, & Stankov (Eds.). Extending intelligence (pp. 319-348). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Salovey, P. and Mayer, J. D. (1990). Emotional intelligence. Imagination, Cognition, and Personality, 9(3), 185-211. Schweizer, K., Moosbrugger, H Schweizer, K., Moosbrugger, H. and Goldhammer, F. (2005). The structure of the relationship between attention and intelligence, Intelligence, 33, 589-611. Spearman, C. (1904). "General intelligence," objectively determined and measured. American Journal of Psychology, 15, 201-293. Sternberg, R. J. and Grigorenko, E. L. (2006). Cultural intelligence and successful intelligence. Group & Organization Management, 31(1), 27-39. Sternberg, R. J. and Detterman, D. K. (eds) (1986) What is Intelligence? Contemporary Viewpoints on its Nature and Definition, Normwood, NJ, Ablex Publishing. Sternberg, R. J. (1999). The theory of successful intelligence, Review of General Psychology, 3, 292-316. Sternberg, R. J. (2003). A Broad View of Intelligence The Theory of Successful Intelligence. Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice & Research, 55(3), 139-154. doi:10.1037/1061-4087.55.3.139. Sternberg, R. J. (1985). Beyond IQ: A Triarchic Theory of Intelligence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Thurstone, L.L. (1938). Primary mental abilities. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.