Aim: This study aimed to look whether prevalence information (details about the frequency) promotes children’s false memories for implausible and plausible events. To look at developmental variations within the development of false …show more content…
memories.
Hypotheses:
1. Children who hear false prevalence information will be more likely to give details of false memories than children without false prevalence information.
2. There will be a difference in the amount of false memories for plausible events compared to implausible events.
3.
Younger children are more expected to develop false memories than older children.
Method:
Participants: 91 primary school children from two different age groups (44 7-8 year olds & 47 11-12 year olds).
Materials: Several pieces of stimulus material were used;
• True narratives describing each Childs first day at school obtained via parents. This acted as a control.
• False narratives, which were developed in a pilot study. 49 children rated the likelihood of an event occurring to them on a 7-point smiley face scale. Depending on the most popular ratings, two events were chosen: almost choked on a candy (plausible) and abducted by UFO (implausible).
• False newspaper articles to act as prevalence information describing either the true narrative or false narrative depending on the condition.
Design: Independent groups design (between-subjects design). The different conditions were:
• Younger children (7-8) vs. older children (11-12)
• Event type: plausible vs. implausible
• Prevalence information: yes vs. no.
Children were randomly assigned to the event type and prevalence …show more content…
information.
Procedure: All children were questioned separately twice over seven days. In both interviews, one true narrative and one false narrative were read clearly and loudly to the children. During both interviews children were instructed to report everything they remembered about the events. In the prevalence information condition, children were told that to assist them recall the event they would be given a news story, read aloud by the interviewer. Children were given retrieval cues such as being told to recall everything and to recreate the context. Independent judges categorized each memory report as no false memory, images but not memories or false memory in order to reduce the effects of demand characteristics, direct response to the interviewer weren’t categorized as a false memory. At the end of Interview 2, children were debriefed for using false memory research. (Otgaar et al., 2009)
Results: The results showed that when children were debriefed they were stunned to hear that the occasions did not really transpire.
For example, one 8-year-old child said, “it really did happen.”
• True events: The children remembered 97% of the true events at interview 1 and 98% of true events during Interview 2.
• False events: At Interview 1, 33% of the children developed a false memory. At Interview 2, 36% of the children developed a false memory.
• Prevalence information: Prevalence information improved the development of 7–8 year old children’s false memories but not 11–12 year old children’s false memories, and this effect occurred at Interview 1, but not at Interview 2. (Otgaar et al., 2009). 7–8 year old children who received prevalence information were approximately two times more likely to report false memories at Interview 1 than their 7–8 year old counterparts who did not receive prevalence information.
• Plausibility: The plausibility of the false event did not affect the development of false memories. That is, both younger and older children were equally likely to report false memories of choking on a candy and being abducted by a UFO.
• Age: The younger children were more likely to report false memories than the older children at Interview
2
Conclusion: The central finding is that combining false descriptions with prevalence information increased the rate of false memory reports in younger children during an initial interview. Furthermore 7-8 year old children were more likely to report false memories about plausible and implausible events than 11-12 year olds. This study demonstrated that children could report false memories about events that are unlikely (over 70%); this was the first study to be successful in promoting false memories in children about implausible UFO abductions.