Underlying Normal Traits within Abnormal Personality Disorders
Student
University
April 11, 2010
Running Head: NORMAL TRAITS WITHIN ABNORMAL PERSONALITY DISORDERS
Abstract
Scholars have argued for decades concerning the fact that there are normal personality traits
underlying abnormal personality traits in people who exhibit dysfunctional personalities. The
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition is the determinative
guide on the descriptions of these personality characteristics, and it determined that there were
several models to be considered when looking for a universal clinical definition of abnormal
personality. …show more content…
Researchers used either the Big Four, Big Five or other models to describe what an
abnormal personality consisted of and how it related to a normal personality as studied.
Researchers measured personality differences based on qualitative, quantitative and other key
factor differences to determine normal or abnormal functioning personalities. It was difficult to
determine one substantive definition, as the traits overlapped from normal to abnormal
characteristics noted. Later, the definition of personality dysfunctions included life skills,
personal tasks and life goals, and whether the individual was able to function as a member of his
society, while meeting the expectations of that society.
A person’s maladaptiveness and
evolutionary sense were added as part of the definition of whether the personality was normal or
abnormal, and whether a person had the skill to be able to manage personal relationships were
considered as well in the general definition of abnormal personality. Today, treatment options
are expanded from the traditional therapy treatments to include drug therapies, psychodynamic
therapy, day hospital intervention, and dialectical behavior therapy. To date, day hospital
interventions have proved very successful on non-schizophrenic patients suffering from
abnormal personality traits.
Running Head: NORMAL TRAITS WITHIN ABNORMAL PERSONALITY DISORDERS
Introduction
Scholars have argued for decades concerning the fact that there are normal personality
traits underlying abnormal personality traits in people who exhibit dysfunctional personalities.
Recently, scholars have begun to make an argument that current category systems of personality
disorders (PDs) should be substituted by trait dimensional scheme designations in the …show more content…
Diagnostic
and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric
Association, 2000). Experts are leaning towards using a Big Four model, which are “essentially
maladaptive variants of the Big Five traits of normal personality, minus Openness” (Watson,
1545). In a discussion of this issue by Watson, Clark and Chmielewski, they state that the newly
comprised Big Four model excludes odd or eccentric Cluster A PDs, (Watson, 1545) and that
their results noted from three studies show a relationship examining the factors of normal and
abnormal personalities. Their results established that the Oddity factor was considered more
broad than the Cluster A traits and more distinct from Openness and other Big Five models,
which suggested “an alternative five factor model of personality pathology (considering only
abnormal traits) and an expanded, integrated Big Six taxonomy that subsumes both normal
and abnormal personality characteristics” (Watson, 1545).
Model Theories
The Watson study explains that the Big Four structure was a result of developed
hierarchical models that combined general models, like the Big Three and the Big Five models.
These former models of personality reviews included multidimensional factors reminiscent of
Running Head: NORMAL TRAITS WITHIN ABNORMAL PERSONALITY DISORDERS
past personality inventories. When the Big Three and Big Five models were formally combined,
it was apparent that “two higher order traits—Neuroticism/Negative Emotionality and
Extraversion/Positive Emotionality—are included in both models” (Watson, 1547). Considering
these changes, Watson proposes a “Big Four” theory which does not include Openness, but does
include many of the traits of the other theories. Watson reports that their research on the Big Five
theory also includes research on a Big Six taxonomy “that subsumes both normal and abnormal
personality dimensions (Watson, 1551).
Definitions of Abnormal Personalities
Researchers have made recent discoveries that “abnormal personalities can be modeled as
extremes of normal personality variation” (O’Connor & Doyce, 2001) (Markon, p. 139). Even
though researchers agree that it is possible to describe normal and abnormal personalities within
the same frameworks, they disagree on the structure of what the framework will encompass.
Even abnormal personality traits are seen now as a variant of the extremes that can happen when
reviewing normal personalities.
One way to make sense of the distinctions between normal and abnormal personalities is
to describe personality disorders (PDs) and develop a working definition for them. By defining
the traits for PDs, the researcher is able to develop a base for delineating personalities studied.
Once normal traits are identified, abnormal traits need to be assessed. This can be done by
reviewing the Big Five model of abnormal personalities. This is the juncture that normal and
abnormal personalities overlap. Apparently, there are similar modeling structures that can be
utilized to describe both normal and abnormal personalities. Some traits are very common
Running Head: NORMAL TRAITS WITHIN ABNORMAL PERSONALITY DISORDERS
between the two models, and others mimic similar personality descriptions.
Meta-analytic Investigation Model
One cohesive factor that applies to both normal and abnormal personalities is the meta-
analytic investigation model. This model was proposed by O’Connor in 2002, and it stated that
there were structural relationships between normal and abnormal personalities (Markon, p. 142).
The O’Connor study in 2002 reviewed 37 personality and psychopathology inventories to
determine if dimensional structure differences existed between clinical and nonclinical
respondents (O’Connor B.P., 2002). O’Connor found similarity between normal and abnormal
populations reviewed and measured similarities “both in the number of factors that exist in the
data matrices and in the factor pattern” (O’Connor B.P., 2002).
The ten abnormal behavior disorders listed by the DSM-IV are listed as: paranoid,
schizoid, schizotypal, antisocial, borderline, histrionic, narcissistic, avoidant, dependent, and
obsessive–compulsive” (Livesley & Jang, p. 258). Each of these disorders shows traits, and it is
the way that professional clinicians are able to make accurate diagnoses of abnormal personality
traits of their patients. This listing of traits by the DSM, showed that the distinction between
what was considered normal and what was considered abnormal was often defined by
distinguishing the “qualitative distinction between the two” (Livesley & Jang, p. 258).
Unfortunately, in truth researchers have come to find out that there are no true separations
between normal and abnormal disorders, and they are hard pressed to find the dividing lines
between the two entities. O’Connor asked whether the distinction can be made using former
Running Head: NORMAL TRAITS WITHIN ABNORMAL PERSONALITY DISORDERS
models, and what exactly was normal or abnormal personality disorder. When the conceptual
distinctions between the two were reviewed, there are several models to note. The most
noteworthy working model being that there was “no evidence of discontinuity in the distributions
of 100 traits selected to provide a systematic representation of personality disorder” (Livesley &
Jang, p.
259). In other words, there was no concrete evidence that the researchers would
consistently find traits that were exclusively common or descriptive of a specific personality
disorder.
In fact, personality disorders were measured across normal and control groups. The
findings were that there were similarities within the disorder traits and that some equaled normal
and others disordered personality traits. In this way, the researchers queried whether disorder
traits could be seen in normal personalities. The answer was that there were few solid
frameworks to make the decision which would provide a definitive answer to the question. In
effect, extreme ends of the traits seemed to be deemed disorders, while extreme variations alone
may not have been considered enough to state that a personality disorder actually existed.
Quantitative Differences in Normal and Abnormal Personalities
Quantitative differences exist between the normal and abnormal personality.
The
differences often mix up and muddle the personality traits and the disorders apparent within
them. With personality disorders, often “it is difficult to see how an extreme score on dimensions
such as conscientiousness, extraversion, or agreeableness is necessarily pathological.
Researchers agreed that there were to be other additional factors that needed be present to justify
the diagnosis (Livesley & Jang, p. 262). That additional trait is inflexibility and subjective
Running Head: NORMAL TRAITS WITHIN ABNORMAL PERSONALITY DISORDERS
distress (Livesley & Jang, p. 259). The character trait of inflexibility is defined as one where the
person has extreme traits, but not necessarily only an extreme position noted on any given trait.
For example, a person who is extremely open and gregarious, but then is not able to tone down
his personality when necessary would be an example of this trait. Continuing with this example,
what would make the person who is considered otherwise outgoing and spontaneous a person
who is suffering from a personality disorder?
Maladaptive Personalities
The answer may come from prior work done by researchers who were determining
personality and abnormal personality disorders. Extreme actions alone were not enough to say
the person operated outside of “normal” personality parameters. The researchers at the time
believed that personality disorders were the result of someone suffering from an abnormal
variation of a personality being studied. It was measured in how much the person suffered from
the disorder. This is where the theory of maladaptation or dyscontrolled impairment came into
play (Widiger & Trull, 1991; Widiger & Sankis, 2000).
The reason the researchers sought a generalized definition is that without one, they
“would have to catalogue the various maladaptive manifestations of each trait” (Livesley & Jang,
p. 263). This was a difficult proposition, since even “normal” people were prone to exhibit
maladaptive traits at some time in their lives. Another problem came with the idea of traits as one
certain set of behaviors that were noted on subjects clinically or otherwise. Extreme exhibitions
of a trait may show some measureable amount of psychopathology, but were not exclusively
indicative of being considered classically maladaptive. In this way, the researchers determined
Running Head: NORMAL TRAITS WITHIN ABNORMAL PERSONALITY DISORDERS
that the “definition of personality disorder needs to incorporate features of disorder that are
separate from, although possibly correlated with, extreme trait variation” (Livesley & Jang, p.
263).
Harmful Dysfunctional Traits in Personalities
These descriptions of personality were necessary because there were more than these
factors to consider when determining a personality disorder. In fact, personality was considered
to be “a system of interrelated structures and processes” (Costa & McCrae, 1994; Mischel, 1999;
Vernon, 1964) which included a person’ dispositional traits, motives, coping mechanisms, and
ability to tame impulses are part of the process of determining normal or abnormal indications of
personality. In other words, if these traits were considered “harmful dysfunctions,” (Wakefield,
1992; Livesley & Jang, p. 263) they consisted of harmful traits that were underlying natural
functions. So, the definition of a personality disorder can be considered a harmful dysfunction in
the normally adaptive functions of a person’s personality system (Livesley & Jang, p. 263)
Another issue within the developing studies of personality disorders was that personality
functions were considered to be seen as disturbed in individuals who exhibited personality
disorders. Researcher Cantor described a person’s personality as the types of tasks a person sets
as personal goals, and they way the person looks at his or her “self, and life situations, and the
strategies used to achieve personal tasks” (Livesley & Jang, p. 263). This delineation of
personality traits offered a true to form definition of what a personality disorder consisted of for
the individual suffering from it. It was considered of a higher order than simply a dysfunction of
a personality trait. Here it was described as needing to concentrate on life tasks as the
Running Head: NORMAL TRAITS WITHIN ABNORMAL PERSONALITY DISORDERS
determining factor to determining if an individual had a personality disorder, and was therefore
considered abnormal in terms of functioning personality.
The researchers assumed that as a person lives his life, he orders his tasks as to what he
sets as priorities for completing goals and meeting the needs of his immediate surrounding
community and culture. This comes under the order of living in society and meeting the
expectations of people who live near the individual, or a way of fitting in within his community.
It also had to do with the person’s mean biology, or biological features characteristic of the
individual. In fact, these tasks did vary depending on where the person lived and what the person
had do to be able to survive in his culture. These may come under the umbrella of life skills, and
they are definitely different considering where a person lived or had grown up. For example, a
person who grew up in a small native Alaskan out island would have different life skills that
would a person who grew up and lived in a borough of Manhattan, NY. The two personalities of
these individuals might be similar, but their life skills would be developed in obviously different
ways. The person living in the native island village would have an understanding of the elements
and what is necessary for bare-bones survival in possibly extreme conditions. While, the person
who grew up in the city would have to understand how to be “street smart” and may need to
know how to survive in even a potentially violent atmosphere if the neighborhood suggested
those skills were essential to survive on a daily basis. Each individual may otherwise be soft
spoken, or be considered similarly warm-hearted or kind. But decidedly, their life skills would
separate them and put them a world away from each other in what they knew and needed to
depend on to survive in their environment on a daily basis.
Running Head: NORMAL TRAITS WITHIN ABNORMAL PERSONALITY DISORDERS
Universal Tasks Underlying Personality Traits
The researchers then understood that there would need to be a set of universal tasks that
needed to be identified. These universal tasks were considered of “evolutionary significance”
and featured four universal challenges as set by Plutchik (1980). These were the four ways a
person’s identity was developed and they included:
the solution to the problems of dominance and submissiveness created by
hierarchy that is characteristic of primate social hierarchies; development
of a sense of territoriality or belongingness; and solution to the problems
of temporality, that is, problems of loss and separation. This allowed the researchers studying personality disorders to come to the conclusion that
personality disorders prevented an individual from managing the adaptive answers or solutions
that were considered universally applicable to everyone, or a person’s life tasks. When an
individual had a deficit in any of these areas, there was a noted “harmful dysfunction” and the
person was unable to adapt to be able to function in his environment or society. The life tasks
then seen as either being fulfilled or being abandoned by the individual, probably because of this
identified deficit. Personality disorder was seen as different from other disorders by the fact that
these failures “should be enduring and traceable to adolescence or at least early adulthood and
they should be due to extreme personality variation rather than another pervasive and chronic
mental disorder such as a cognitive or schizophrenic disorder” (Livesley & Jang, p. 264).
Running Head: NORMAL TRAITS WITHIN ABNORMAL PERSONALITY DISORDERS
Evolutionary Sense Within Personality Traits There was talk of the individual not being able to adapt to his environment in an
“evolutionary sense” which spoke to whether the person had garnered enough skills for ensuring
adaptive social behavior to allow reproduction and survival (Livesley & Jang, p. 264). This was
explained as stating that the adaptive traits would contribute to the person adapting to his
environment and society in general, and the person adapting to his family unit would move the
person towards being able to rear children and eventually reproduce to pass down his traits to
offspring later on. This is the general definition of people who have self confidence in their
dealings with others, and are able to live in harmony in stable relationships, while becoming
productive members within their society or community. These can be seen to be part of the
ancestral or evolutionary needs of every individual, whether the person had an abnormal
personality or normal personality.
The more common description of an abnormal personality comes from what the common
person observes when someone has problems dealing within a relationship. Rutter (1987) stated
that personality disorders were characterized by “persistent, pervasive abnormality in social
relationships and social functioning generally” (Rutter, p. 454). Also, Tyrer (2001) stated that
“we do not necessarily need to know everything about someone's personality to recognise the
elements that make it disordered” (Tyrer, p. 83). Tyrer states quite honestly that psychiatrists
view these descriptive axioms as something to be deferred, and says “personality disorder and
mental retardation are stigmatic terms that psychiatrists like to avoid” (Tyrer, p. 83).
So the question is, how can one determine the underlying normality within the abnormal
Running Head: NORMAL TRAITS WITHIN ABNORMAL PERSONALITY DISORDERS
personality? For this the clinician and the layperson need review the DSM-III, considered the
premier source of personality disorder classification (American Psychiatric Association, 1980).
The DSM classifies what is considered normal and abnormal in terms of personality. The
professionals in the field disagree to the proposed stereotyping of this group of classifications, on
the basis of the fact that such profiling is considered “quite inappropriate in such a complicated
field” (Tyrer, p. 84). In fact, it appears that there are burgeoning alternative and substitute
classifications being used for determining personality disorders in surveys, trials, studies and
private practice. Most people would be surprised to find out that this topic has been heatedly
debated over the past two decades. Many people most likely assume that there is one clinical
definition of what is normal, and what is not normal when it comes to personality disorders. The
media plays into this, as well as the television and movie plots. The person seen as abnormal is
cloaked in symbolic black, speaks in a raspy voice or has otherwise obvious mentally deviant
behaviors that even the least sophisticated person in the audience could confidently label as the
“bad guy.”
Personality Disorders Studied Abroad
Even the study of personality disorders abroad have led researchers to agree to disagree in
the area of determining how to describe profiles for patients with underlying normal traits within
their abnormal personality profiles. In a study performed by McCrae (2001) in The People’s
Republic of China, 1,909 psychiatric patients were examined to determine the accuracy of the
hypotheses determined from the Interpretive Report of the Revised NEO Personality Inventory
(McCrae, p. 155). The researchers determined that the PDs were not separate categories that
Running Head: NORMAL TRAITS WITHIN ABNORMAL PERSONALITY DISORDERS
could be determined in a vacuum. They realized that they needed to consider a more
comprehensive and forgiving system of personality traits, to be considered an accurate measure
of the patient’s personality issues and concerns.
The researchers found that the personality traits of the patients did not fit into the DSM-IV
defined traits. They did “draw on the same five underlying personality traits” (McCrae, p. 171),
and were considered redundant, but there were several areas of overlap to be considered
conclusive. In fact, over 60% of the patients that were being treated for maladaptive personalities
were not meeting the criteria defined in the DSM-IV, as relating to any criteria for a PD (McCrae,
p. 171). The maladaptive behaviors, the person’s habits and personal attitudes were all measured
to find a comprehensive scale for measuring the personality traits of the patients. It was
determined that the results were insignificant, and concluded that personality profiles were
“modest predictors of categorical PDs, but they are immensely informative about people”
(McCrae, p. 172).
Treatment Options for Abnormal Personality Traits
But clinicians and psychiatrists are still interested in treating and helping people who
exhibit the traits of these personality disorders identified above. They are in disagreement
whether there are normal traits that are underlying the abnormal personality traits that deserve to
be treated in an effort to offer the patient an opportunity to live a full and productive life. This is
a critical option for people who have normal personality traits, but also exhibit the identified
borderline abnormal personality traits as well within their psyche.
Over a half decade ago, the best treatments were heralded as therapeutic, and they seemed
Running Head: NORMAL TRAITS WITHIN ABNORMAL PERSONALITY DISORDERS
to promise the greatest success overall. But today, there are many alternate treatments available
for individuals exhibiting abnormal personality disorders. They include drug therapies,
psychodynamic therapy, day hospital intervention, and dialectical behavior therapy (Linehan,
1992, Tyrer, p. 84). Other methods of treatment that carry high success for the patients are the
partial hospitalization of patients (Bateman & Fonagy, 1999). Bateman & Fonagy compared the
effectiveness of treating patients exhibiting borderline personality disorders with partial
hospitalization s a standard psychiatric care. They studied thirty-eight patients with borderline
personality disorder and offered them individual and group psychoanalytic psychotherapy, for up
to 18 months (Bateman & Fonagy, 1999). The results were that the patients who had been
partially hospitalized did exhibit less problems, with “An improvement in depressive symptoms,
a decrease in suicidal and self-mutilatory acts, reduced inpatient days, and better social and
interpersonal function began at 6 months and continued until the end of treatment at 18 months”
(Bateman & Fonagy, 1999). Their conclusion was that the partial hospitalization was determined
as a far superior type of psychiatric care for those patients exhibiting borderline personality
disorder. This treatment option was in opposition with the standard treatment options of the
therapies listed above. These results were similar in the study by Piper, (1993) where a day
treatment program at the University of Alberta Hospital in Edmonton, Alberta was studied. The
patients were referred from the day treatment program and walk-in clinic, and utilized
participants with “chronically disturbed non-schizophrenic patients, who usually have affective
and personality disorders” (Piper, p. 757). The results of the study were that day treatment
programs were considered effective for patients with long-term nonschizophrenic disorders. The
Running Head: NORMAL TRAITS WITHIN ABNORMAL PERSONALITY DISORDERS
patients noted significant improvement in “four of the five areas studied—interpersonal
functioning, symptomatology, life satisfaction, and self-esteem—as well as in several of
disturbance associated with individual objectives (Piper, p. 762).
Reference
American Psychiatric Association (1980) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders
(3rd edn) (DSM—III). Washington, DC: APA.
Bateman, A. & Fonagy, P. (1999). Effectiveness of partial hospitalization in the treatment of
borderline personality disorder: a randomized controlled trial. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 156, 1563-1569. Retrieved on April 9, 2010 from
http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org/cgi/content/full/156/10/1563?ijkey=bb19a5d116af525fe927da3b0a0c0250f3d61de3
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1994). Can personality change? In T. F. Heatherton, & J. L.
Weinberger (Eds.), Can personality change? (pp. 21–40). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Linehan, M. M. (1992) Cognitive Therapy for Borderline Personality Disorder. New York:
Guilford Press.
Livesley, W. John & Jang, Kerry L.. (2005). Differentiating normal, abnormal, and disordered
personality, European Journal of Personality, 19(4), 257-268.
Markon, K.E, Krueger, R. F., & Watson, D. (2005). Delineating the structure of normal and
abnormal personality: An integrative hierarchical approach. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 88, 139–157. EBSCO Database: Academic Search Premier.
McCrae, Robert R., Jian, Yang, et al. (2001). Personality Profiles and the Prediction of
Categorical Personality Disorders. Journal of Personality, 69(2), 155-174.
Mischel, W. (1999). Personality coherence and dispositions in a Cognitive–Affective Personality
System (CAPS) approach. In D. Cervone, & Y. Shoda (Eds.), The coherence of
personality (pp. 37–60). New York: Guilford.
O’Connor B.P. (2002). The search for dimensional structure differences between normality and
abnormality: A statistical review of published data on personality and psychopathology.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 83(4), 962–982. Retrieved on April 9,
2010 from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12374447
O’Connor B.P. & Dyce J.A. (2001) Rigid and extreme: A geometric representation of
personality disorders in five-factor model space. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 81, 1119–1130. PubMed Database.
Piper, W.E., Rosie, J.S., Azim, H.F.A, Joyce A.S. (1993). A randomized trial of psychiatric day
treatment for patients with affective and personality disorders. Hosp Community
Psychiatry, 44, 757–763.
Plutchik, R. (1980). A general psychoevolutionary theory of emotion. In R. Plutchik, & H.
Kellerman (Eds.), Emotion: Theory, research, and experience (pp. 3–33). San Diego, CA:
Academic.
Rutter, M. (1987). Temperament, personality and personality disorder. British Journal of
Psychiatry, 150, 443–458.
Tyrer, Peter. (2001). Personality disorder. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 179, 81-84.
Retrieved on April 9, 2010 from http://bjp.rcpsych.org/cgi/content/full/179/1/81
Watson, David, Clark, Lee Anna, Chmielewski, Michael. (2008). Structures of Personality and
Their Relevance to Psychopathology: II. Further Articulation of a Comprehensive
Unified Trait Structure. Journal of Personality, 76(6), 1545-1586. EBSCO Database:
Academic Search Premier.
Vernon, P. E. (1964). Personality assessment: A critical survey. London: Methuen.
Wakefield, J. C. (1992). Disorder as harmful dysfunction: A conceptual critique of DSM-III-R’s
definition of mental disorder. Psychological Review, 99, 232–247.
Widiger, T. A., & Sankis, L. M. (2000). Adult psychopathology: Issues and controversies.
Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 377–404.
Widiger, T. A., & Trull, T. J. (1991). Diagnosis and clinical assessment. Annual Review of
Psychology, 42, 109–133.