For more then a century, psychologists have desperately sought to have their disciplines accepted. Psychology requires some degree of trickery in the experimental set-up. But how much insight do we then gain into how people will behave outside the laboratory? And if the experimental method we wish to use is sufficiently destructive to prevent us using it on humans, what do we learn from studying on animals? A description of psychology experiments has drawn many criticisms. These and other details where looked at in chapter 1, where Slater goes back in time and looks at B.F. Skinner famous experiment called "the rat race". Skinner performed an experiment on rats using food levers and other instruments to condition and shape their behavior, to see if we organisms are easily programmed to obedience or we are all at our free will. Skinner believed through his experiments that we would obey the rules like computers.
Slater came across as an eccentric person who had a great courage and passion for psychology to try and solve the most controversial …show more content…
issues. Her love for psychology dates to her very early age where she always had a curiosity to search for answers in the open ended theories.
As part of Slater story of Stanley Milgram's famous obedience experiments, where a large proportion of people where shown to be enable to resist instructors to deliver what they thought to be lethal electric shocks to their fellow men.
Another engaging chapters in the Skinner's box was the one in 1972 experiment of David Rosenhan, who recruited several participants to visit psychiatric emergency rooms with a single complaint: "I am hearing a voice. It is saying, 'Thud.'" They were to present no other remarkable symptoms or behaviour. Rosenhan's participants were allegedly all admitted to inpatient units and diagnosed as mentally ill. Celebrants of Rosenhan's study have a certain self-righteous criticism on psychiatry as a disciplinable to differentiate between illness and health, between insanity and sanity. Slater joins the rank and repeats a version of Rosenhan's study herself, in which she reports to various emergency rooms with the same single complaint. Slater on the other hand was treated with kindness and respect, after a consultation with a psychiatrist, she was released, but the downside of it was only after 12 minutes of doctor's time she was given a prescriptions for anti-psychotic and anti-depressant drugs.
Psychiatry and psychiatrists, by necessity, continue to refine diagnosis and treatment. The fact that the system is flawed translates into continued effort to improve our tools, and the fact that the system can be fooled seems to be only a superficial satisfaction for those who wish to criticize the field. Psychiatry will continue to evolve, along with its imperfections as a clinical field. Evolution, after all, is a progress not to perfection but to improvement.
Take, for example, the work of John Darley and Bibb Latan, psychologists who devised a series of experiments to test why it is that people sometimes ignore other people's calls for help, and why, at other times, we will leap to others' comfort.
Darley and Latan's experiments were inspired by the gruesome murder and rape of Kitty Genovese, a crime that took place over a 35-minute period in the predawn hours of March 13, 1964, in a working-class section of Queens, New York. Thirty-eight people witnessed the murder and rape, and nobody called the police for help while it was occurring. Thirty-eight people, why were they all so
heartless?
But they were not heartless, of course. They were human. In a series of experiments on New York University students, Darley and Latan discovered the phenomenon of "diffusion of responsibility" the more people who witness an event, Slater writes, "the less responsible any one individual feels and, indeed, is, because responsibility is evenly distributed among the crowd." Combined with social norms who want to be the first one to make a fuss if nobody else seems to be too upset?
Diffusion of responsibility can paralyze a crowd. People witnessing a crime or any other kind of emergency will do nothing.
In one experiment, they put a naive subject into a room with three actors. They told all four to fill out a questionnaire on college life. After several minutes, the psychologists began to release a non-hazardous white smoke into room through an air vent. The three actors, who'd been instructed to act normally, continued filling out their forms. And what did the fourth person, the experiment's subject, do? "The smoke started pouring like cream, coming faster, heavier, smearing the air and blotting out figures, faces," Slater writes. "Each time, the subject looked alarmed, looked at the smoke going from wisp to waft, looked at the calm confederates, and then, clearly confused, went back to filling out the questionnaire." In the entire experiment, only four subjects ever reported the smoke everyone else stuck with the questionnaire.
Most of us would like to believe we're somehow above, or beyond, our psychology. We would have called the police on Genovese murder, though those 38 did not. We would have alerted the experimenters to the smoke in the room, though most others did not. We would not have electrocuted an innocent man just because an authority figure had instructed us to do so, as 65 percent of the people in Stanley Milgram's infamous Yale experiment. But really who are we kidding? We would not have called the police to save any of those people. And there are experiments to prove it.
I was unfamiliar to all that occurred in psychology during the last century and Slater's explorations was especially interesting to me. I believe that this book Opening Skinner's box has to be read. Slater discovered that this book has opened a new gateways and doors to another domain.