ED 307 684
AUTHOR
TITLE
PUB DATE
NOTE
PUB TYPE
EDRS PRICE
DESCRIPTORS
IDENTIFIERS
EA 020 980
Grissom, James B.; Shepard, Lorrie A.
Structural Equation Modeling of Retention and Overage
Effects on Dropping Out of School.
Mar 89
22p.; Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the
American Educational Research Association (San
Francisco, CA, March 27-31, 1989).
Speecheg/Conference Papers (150) -- Reports Research /Technical (143)
MF01/PC01 Plus Postage.
Academic Failure; *Dropout Characteristics; Dropout
Programs; *Dropout Rate; Dropout Research; Elementary
Secondary Education; *Grade Repetition; School
Districts; *Socioeconomic Influences;
Underachievement
Illinois (Chicago); Texas (Austin)
ABSTRACT …show more content…
This study addresses the effect that grade retention has on-dropping out of school. ;1 structural model was developed to test the effect of grade retention on dropping out while controlling fcr the effects of other possible mediating variables, especiall.
a..:hieement. This model with slight modifications was applied across four different school districts: Austin, Chicago, and two unnamed districts, one a high socioeconomic status suburban district in the
Northeast, and the other a large urban district in the Southwest.
Conclusions suggest that a causal connection between retention and dropping out exists; however, the model does not explain why retained students are more likely to drop out. Appended are 10 references and
5 figures. (SI)
Reproductions supplied by EDRS are the best that can be made from the original document.
Structural Equation Modeling of Retention and Overage
Effects on Dropping Out of School'
James B. Grissom
Lorrie A. Shepard
U S DEPARTMENT OF EDUCATION
Once of Educational Research and Improvement
"PERMISSION TO REPRODUCE THIS
RIAL HAS BEEN GRANTED CY
MA
FOUCATIONAL RESOURCES INFORMATION
CENTER (ERIC)
document has been reproduced as rece.ved from the person or organization oppinalingit K,
C Minor changes have been made to .mprove rePrOduction Quality
Points of view or opinions stated ,n thrs docu
TO THE EDUCATIONAL RESOURCES
INFORMATION CENTER (ERIC)"
ment do nol necessarily represent off.cat
OERI position 0, policy
'Paper pr4esent.,--1 at the annual meeting of the American
Educational Research Association, San Francisco, March 1989
2
BEST COPY AVAILABLE
t
Abstract
Studies of dropouts consistently find that repeating a grade is associated with drc ping out of school.
Since prac-
tical constraints prohibit using an experimental design, a
structural model was developed to test the effect of grade retention on dropping out while controlling for the effects of other possible mediating variables, especially achievement.
This model with slight modifications was applied
across four different school districts, referred to as
Samples 1 through 4.
In each district, grade retention had a sizable effect on dropping out.
In Sample 1 retained students were more likely
to drop out of school by an increase of 27 percentage points over students not retained.
In Samples 2 and 3 retained stu-
dents were more likely to drop out by and increase of 17 percentage points.
In Sample 4, students in the 1979 freshman
class and the 1981 freehman class had, respectively, 14 and
18 percentage point increases in the likelihood of dropping out over etudents not retained.
The incremental increase
from 14 to 18 percent represents the effect of a stricter eighth grade promotion policy intended to reduce the dropout rate by improving achievement.
The model does not explain how grade retention increases the likelihood that students will leave …show more content…
school.
Dropping out
is a complex process and grade retention contributes to this process in ways that still need to be clarified.
3
It is
clear, however, that repeating a grade contributes to this process in a way that is distinct from the effects of poor achievement. 4
1
Introduction
Many educators believe that passing students on to the next grade before they have demonstrated some minimum
It is harmful because it
level of competency is harmful.
is assumed that students, lagging behind their age-normal
If passed on, these
peers, are experiencing failure.
'slower' students will continue to fail academically unless they are given the opportunity to catch up.
Catch-
t retaining them in the same
ing students up has often m.
grade for another year of school.
Despite a growing body
of research showing that retention does not improve students' achievement (Abidin, Golladay, & Howerton 1971;
Holmes & Matthews, 1984), it remains an acceptable educaRetention remains acceptable because
tional practice.
practitioners either aren't aware of or don't believe the research evidence.
Typically teachers observe that
retained students do better academically during the retained year.
Teacher observations, however, are short
term and lack comparison to poor achieving students who are promoted.
Are there long term consequences of grade
retention that teachers never sae and that are quite different from their perceptions?
This study addresses the
effect that grade retention has on dropping out of school.
Although an association between repeating a grade and dropping out is frequently noted in the research on dropouts, it is rarely mentioned by those who study the
5
2
effect! of nonpromotion.
Furthermore, the apparent asso-
ciation is often dismissed because poor achievement is thought to explain both retention and dropping out.
Practical constraints prohibit testing the effect of grade retention on dropping out using an experimental design. However, a few studies, in which achievement and
retention have been examined concurrently, imply that cer-
tain variables, particularly grade retention, might be causal in their relationship to dropping out (Hese &
Lauber, 1985;
Lloyd, 1978;
& Foster, 1987).
Rice, Toles, Schulz, Harvey,
Therefore, a structural model was devel-
oped representing the hypothesized relationship among available variables that lead to dropping out of school.
This model, with slight modifications, was applied across four different school districts, Austin, Chicago, and two unnamed districts, one a high socio-econcmic district in
the northeast, the other a large urban district in the southwest. Sample 1: The Austin Independent School District
Austin is a large urban school district in Texas with an approximate student enrollment of 60,000.
Data were
supplied by the office of Research and Evaluation for
29,399 7th through 12th graders from the 1984-85 school year. Each student's completion status was updated
through 1987.
All students were classified as dropouts,
6
3
graduates, or currently enrolled, depending on their 1987 status. Graduates and currently enrolled students were
combined for purposes of analysis.
Information was provided on students' sex, ethnic
group, and eligibility for a subsidized lunch, used as an index for socio-economic status (SES).
Achievement scores
were available for the Iowa Test of Basic Skills (ITBS) and the Tests of Achievement and Proficiency (TAP).
With-
in-grade percentiles scores were converted to normal curve equivalents (NCEs).
NCEs were averaged across the years
available for each student on each subtest.
Than, confir-
matory factor analysis was used to estimate an overall achievement score, separately for the ITBS and the TAP.
Grade retention information was available for a seven year period, 1980-81 to 1986-87.
Therefore retentions prior to
1980-81 had t-) be inferred from age.
Normal 7..4e for grade
was determined by historic entrance age policies.
The question arises as to whether or not overage is an accurate indicator of retention.
Because Texas has one of
the earliest entrance ages in the nation (i.e. five by
Laptember 1), it is not likely that students could be too old for their grade by moving to Texas from another state.
However children could be too old without having repeated,
if their parents held them out of school for an extra year. Based on kindergarten entrance practices elsewhere,
it is most likely that parents would elec.: to hold their
children out when their birthdays are within three molzhs
7
4
of the cutoff (Shepard & Smith, 1985).
Therefore, it was
decided to define overage in two ways, one using the exact
September cutoff and another in which students within the ambiguous three months before September were removed.
The
later will be referred to as tne exaggerated definition of overage. Analyses were conducted using the exact defini-
tion of overage, the exaggerated definition of overage,
The
verified -.7etentions, and verified plus exact overage.
most defensible definition is the one in which verified retentions are combined with the exact overage because it helps account for some of the inaccuracies of both.
For
example, it helps identify certain students who have been retained but who are not overage. (These are students who have moved to Texas from a district witn a cutoff date later than September 1, who have birthdays after September
1,
and who have been retained in Texas after 1980-81.)
It
does not correctly classify those who are overage but have not been retained.
However, misidentifying overage stu-
dents as retained should reduce rather than inflate apparent effects.
Results
Figure 1 shows verified retentions combined with the exact definition of overage and the ITBS as the measure achievement. In this figure the path coefficient from
retention to dropping out is .34.
However retention and
dropping out are both dichotomous variables making the
O
5
The unstandar-
path coefficient difficult to interpret.
dized regression coefficient can help with interpretation since it can be read as a probability.
The unstandardized
coefficient (the figure 1 values in parentheses) is .27.
This means that students who were retained were more likely to drop out of school by an increase of 27 percent-
age points over students not retained holding all of the other variables constant.
When the TAP is used to measure
achievement these values become .39 (.29).
Again using
the ITBS to measure achievement but changing the defini-
tion of retention to the exact definition of overage these values become .32 (.29).
For the exaggerated definition
of overage and for verified retentions these values become
.34 (.35) and .34 (.29) respectively.
The direct effect
of achievement is -.07, meaning for every standard unit increase in achievement dropping out decreased .07 standard units.
The total effect of achievement is -.22.
Although the direct paths of the three exogenous variables ti.e. SES, sex, and ethni:ity) show little effect, their
direction indicates that students receiving a subsidized lunch are 3% more likely to drop out than students not receiving such a lunch, females are 2% more likely to drop out than males and Anglos are 3% more likely to drop out
-
than minority students.
Despite how retention or achieve-
ment are defined, retention always has a greater effect on dropping out than any of tha other variables.
9
6
Sample 2: High socio-economic district
Sample 2 is from a large suburban school district in the northeast.
This district was asked to participate
because its high average socio-economic level permitted testing the effect of retention on dropping out in quite different circumstances.
It has a total minority popula-
tion of only 20% compared to 4E% for Austin and 80% in each of the two other districts.
It also has an official
dropout rate of only 4% compared to 20-24% for Austin.
It
was hypothesized that retention might not have an effect on dropping out when support systems exist to minimize its negative impact.
Parents in a high socio-economic dis-
trict are more likely to be supportive and involved in
their children's schooling and advocates believe that retention is more likely to be successful in such an environment.
Data were provided by the Research and Testing office for 38,364 7th through 12th graders in 1985-86.
Grade
ret,mtion and dropout data were updated for the next two years. Information was supplied on sex and ethnicity.
Achievement was estimated by averaging standardized total battery scores on the California Achievement Test (CA2) for eighth and eleventh grades.
As with Austin, retention had to be inferred f-om overage. However, this district like many districts in
the northeast has a late entrance cutoff of December 31.
10
7
Therefore, when overage is defined precisely at this boundary, there will be many students with September to
December birthdays who appear to be too old but were never retained. A student who transferred here from Texas, with
a September to December birthday, but making normal progress would appear as overage.
Given the potential for
misclassifying students who have moved here from another region of the country, the exaggerated definition of overage represents the more reasonable estimate.
In this
case, students with birth dates from September 1 through
December 31 were removed from the analysis to create the exaggerated definition of overage.
This definition of
overage was combined with known retentions, for the three most recent years, to create the variable that defines grade reteation.
Results
Figure 2 shows the path coefficient from retention to dropping out is .29.
.17.
The unstandardized coefficient is
If only students in grades 10-12 are included,
because only these students have had a chance to graduate
(or be enrolled in their senior year), then the retentiondropout relation becomes .34 (.22).
Despite the effects
of a more advantaged family setting and community support to stay in school retained students are approximately 20% more likely to drop colt of school than students not
retained holding achievement and other variables constant.
11
8
Sample 3: Southwest Urban District
The data were provided by the Research and Evaluation office for a large urban district in the southwest. tape was created from two sources of information.
A
The
first source of information was a random sample of
24,844 6th, 8th and 10th grade students in 1985-86.
This
sample of students was drawn from a sample of schools to provide information for an integration study.
Cumula-
tive records, containing information on sex, ethnicity, achievement scores on the Survey of Essential Skills (SES)
and the Comprehensive Tests of Basic Skills (CTBS), and grade level information back to 1979-1980 were recorded and updated in 1986-87.
The second source of information
was the district reported dropouts in 1985-86 and 1986-87.
Dropouts were identified in the integration study sample by name, date of birth, and sex.
It was decided to use only verified retentions in this
study, even though the cumulative records for the 1985-86
10th graders were incomplete below 1979-1980.
There was
interest in testing the effect of grade retention when retention was not confounded by misclassified overage students.
Results
The analysis was conducted only for the 1985-86 10th
1.2
9
graders, a sample of 7,125 students.
The other two
classes were only in 9th and 7th grades in 1986-87, too young to have adequate numbers of dropouts.
Figure 3
shows that wh n the CTBS was used to estimate achievement the path coefficient from grade retention to dropping out
The unstandardized coefficient iq also .17.
is .17.
When
the SES was used to estimate achievement the path coefficient and the unstandardized regression coefficient from retention to dropping out are .19 and .21 respectively.
As in sample 2 retained students are more likely to 1Fave school early by an increase of approximately 20 percentage point over students not retained holding the other vari-
ables constant.
Sample 4:
Chicago Public Schools
Chicago is a very large urban school system.
Its
populatio, is 80% minority with longitudinal dropout rates of 40-45%
The data provided by the Department of Research
and Evaluation is the same data analyzed by Rice, Toles,
Schulz, Harvey, and Foster (1987).
These data were ana-
lyzed by Rice et al. to study the effect of a stricter 8th grade promotion policy initiated in 1980 and intended to improve graduation rates by improving achievement.
How-
ever, in our study, Asians and students with missing achievement scores were omitted leaving a sample of 63,872
13
10 students. This sample represents three cohorts of stu-
dents, the 1979, 1980, and 1981 freshmen classes.
Information on sex and ethnicity were provided along
with reading scores on the ITBS administered in the 8th grade. (Reading scores were categorized as below grade
level (< 7.1), at grade level (7.1-8.0), and above grade level (8.1 +).'
Retention was represented by Rice et al.
as overage fQr grade.
Chicago has a December 1 entrance
cutoff date and therefore there should be some ambiguity in using overage as a proxy for retention.
Because birth
date information was not available it was not possible to experiment with a more stringent definition of overage.
Results
The best way to look at these data is to compare the effect of retention on dropping out for 1979 and 1981 freshmen. See figures 4 and S.
The 1979 class was the
last to be unaffected by the stricter enforcement of 8th grade promotion standards.
The 1981 class includes the
first group of students made to repeat 8th grade for another year.
For the 1979 class the effect of retention
on dropping out is .13 (.14). class is .18 (.18).
The effect for the 1981
Rather than reducing the rate of
dropping out, the stricter promotion policy increased retained students chances of dropping out by 4 percentage points. n
Conclusions and Discussion
While acknowleth.ding that structural models can never
prove causal relationships our analyses suggest that,
after adjusting for achievement and various other background variables, there may indeed be a causal connection from retention to dropping out.
However the model does
not explain why retained students are more likely to drop gut. Grade retention undoubtedly contributes in subtle
and interactive way:: to an already complex set of causes for school leaving.
For example, repeating a grade might
contribute to some negative set of school experiences that work to convince students that they can't succeed.
Repeating a grade might contribute to a sense of alienation by making students feel too old for their classmates or feel too old to still be in high school.
Being older
might also make students less willing to wait to take on adult roles such as getting a job or getting married.
Or
more likely it is a combination of these or other processes since they are not mutually exclusive.
In any
case, the negative consequences of making students repeat a year of school are clear.
15
/2,
Bibliograph:,
Abidin, R. R., Jr., Golladay, W. M., & Howerton, A. L.
(1971).
Elementary school retention:
An unjustifiable,
discriminatory and noxious educational policy.
Journal of
School Psychology, 9, 410-417.
Structural equation modeling
Grissom, J. B. (1988).
of retention and overage effects on dropping out of school. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of
Colorado, Boulder.
Grissom, J. B. & Shepard, L. A. dropping out of school.
Repeating and
A. Shepard & M. L. Smith
In L
Research and policies on reten-
(Eds.), Flunkirg grades: tion. (1989).
Lewes: The Falmer Press.
Hess, G. A., & Lauber, D.
Dropouts from the
An analysis of the classes of
Chicago Public Schools:
1982, 1983, 1984.
(1985).
Chicago Panel on Public School Finance.
(ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 258 095)
Holm s, C. T,, & Matthews, K. M. (1984).
The effects
of nonpromet.ion on elementary and junior high school pupils: Review of Educational Research,
A meta-analysis.
54, 225-236.
Lloyd, D. N. (1978).
from third-grade data.
Prediction of school failure
Educational and Psychological
Measurement, 38, 1193-1200.
16
13
Rice, W. K., Toles, R. E., Schulz, E. M., Harvey, J.
T., & Foster, D. L.
A low:itudinal investigation
(1987).
of effectiveness on increased promotion standards at eighth grade of high school graduation.
A paper presented
at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research
Association, Washington DC.
Schulz, E. M., Toles, R., Rice, W. K., Brauer, I.,
Harvey, J. (1986).
&
Association of dropout rates with
student attributes.
Paper presented at the annual meeting
of the American Educational Research Association, San
Francisco, California.
Shepard, L. A., & Smith, M. L. (1985, March).
Retention practices and reten-
Valley kindergarten study: tion effects.
Boulder
Boulder, CO:
Boulder Valley Public
Schools.
Smith, M. L., & Shepard, L. A. (1988). readiness and retention: beliefs and practices.
Kindergarten
A qualitative study of teachers'
American Educational Research
Journal, 25, 307-333.
17
A4 (A3)
SES
A3 (02)
-.07
.43
RETAIN
SEX
.34 (.27)
DROPOUT
.07
ETHNIC
U2
U2
.84
.00(00)
-A4 (-.03)
Figure 1
Path diagram of Subgroup 1 (ITBS) in Sample 1 (Austin).
Retention was estimated by adding students who had verified grade repetitions to those who were overage as defined by the exact cutoff.
18
Figure 2
Path diagram of Sample 2 (high SES district).
Retention was estimated by adding students who had verified grade repetitions to those who were overage as defined by the exaggerated cutoff.
19
00 .0
-.o.4 (-.o.))
.10
1
i'
ACHtEv
.15
ErviblIC
Figure 3
Path diagram for Sample 3
CTBS used to measure achievement
20
-.10 (-.10)
-.08 (-An
SEX
N
-.29
RETAIN
.13 (.14)
DROPOU r
1.96
U2
ETHNIC
Figure
-.01 (-.01)
-.01 (-.01)
Path diagram of Cohort 1 (1979 freshmen) in Sample '
(Chicago).
21
-.11 (-.11)
-.10 (-.10)
SEX
.04
ACHIEV
-33
si RETAIN
.18 (.18)
DROPOUT
.94
.94
.98
U3
ETHNIC
Figure 5
.01 (.01)
-.04 (-.05)
Path diagram of Cohort 3 (1981 freshmen) in Sample 4
(Chicago).
P2