Top-Rated Free Essay
Preview

The Sapir Whorf Hypothesis

Powerful Essays
2078 Words
Grammar
Grammar
Plagiarism
Plagiarism
Writing
Writing
Score
Score
The Sapir Whorf Hypothesis
Within linguistic theory, two extreme positions concerning the relationship between language and thought are commonly referred to as 'mould theories’ and 'cloak theories '. Mould theories represent language as 'a mould in terms of which thought categories are cast ' (Bruner et al. 1956, p. 11). Cloak theories represent the view that 'language is a cloak conforming to the customary categories of thought of its speakers ' (ibid.). The doctrine that language is the 'dress of thought ' was fundamental in Neo-Classical literary theory (Abrams 1953, p. 290), but was rejected by the Romantics (ibid.; Stone 1967, Ch. 5). There is also a related view (held by behaviourists, for instance) that language and thought are identical. According to this stance thinking is entirely linguistic: there is no 'non-verbal thought ', no 'translation ' at all from thought to language. In this sense, thought is seen as completely determined by language.

The Sapir-Whorf theory, named after the American linguists Edward Sapir and Benjamin Lee Whorf, is a mould theory of language. Writing in 1929, Sapir argued in a classic passage that:

Human beings do not live in the objective world alone, nor alone in the world of social activity as ordinarily understood, but are very much at the mercy of the particular language which has become the medium of expression for their society. It is quite an illusion to imagine that one adjusts to reality essentially without the use of language and that language is merely an incidental means of solving specific problems of communication or reflection. The fact of the matter is that the 'real world ' is to a large extent unconsciously built upon the language habits of the group. No two languages are ever sufficiently similar to be considered as representing the same social reality. The worlds in which different societies live are distinct worlds, not merely the same world with different labels attached... We see and hear and otherwise experience very largely as we do because the language habits of our community predispose certain choices of interpretation. (Sapir 1958 [1929], p. 69)
This position was extended in the 1930s by his student Whorf, who, in another widely cited passage, declared that:
We dissect nature along lines laid down by our native languages. The categories and types that we isolate from the world of phenomena we do not find there because they stare every observer in the face; on the contrary, the world is presented in a kaleidoscopic flux of impressions which has to be organized by our minds - and this means largely by the linguistic systems in our minds. We cut nature up, organize it into concepts, and ascribe significances as we do, largely because we are parties to an agreement to organize it in this way - an agreement that holds throughout our speech community and is codified in the patterns of our language. The agreement is, of course, an implicit and unstated one, but its terms are absolutely obligatory; we cannot talk at all except by subscribing to the organization and classification of data which the agreement decrees. (Whorf 1940, pp. 213-14; his emphasis)
I will not attempt to untangle the details of the personal standpoints of Sapir and Whorf on the degree of determinism which they felt was involved, although I think that the above extracts give a fair idea of what these were. I should note that Whorf distanced himself from the behaviourist stance that thinking is entirely linguistic (Whorf 1956, p. 66). In its most extreme version 'the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis ' can be described as consisting of two associated principles. According to the first, linguistic determinism, our thinking is determined by language. According to the second, linguistic relativity, people who speak different languages perceive and think about the world quite differently.
On this basis, the Whorfian perspective is that translation between one language and another is at the very least, problematic, and sometimes impossible. Some commentators also apply this to the 'translation ' of unverbalized thought into language. Others suggest that even within a single language any reformulation of words has implications for meaning, however subtle. George Steiner (1975) has argued that any act of human communication can be seen as involving a kind of translation, so the potential scope of Whorfianism is very broad indeed. Indeed, seeing reading as a kind of translation is a useful reminder of the reductionism of representing textual reformulation simply as a determinate 'change of meaning ', since meaning does not reside in the text, but is generated by interpretation. According to the Whorfian stance, 'content ' is bound up with linguistic 'form ', and the use of the medium contributes to shaping the meaning. In common usage, we often talk of different verbal formulations 'meaning the same thing '. But for those of a Whorfian persuasion, such as the literary theorist Stanley Fish, 'it is impossible to mean the same thing in two (or more) different ways ' (Fish 1980, p. 32). Reformulating something transforms the ways in which meanings may be made with it, and in this sense, form and content are inseparable. From this stance words are not merely the 'dress ' of thought.

The importance of what is 'lost in translation ' varies, of course. The issue is usually considered most important in literary writing. It is illuminating to note how one poet felt about the translation of his poems from the original Spanish into other European languages (Whorf himself did not in fact regard European languages as significantly different from each other). Pablo Neruda noted that the best translations of his own poems were Italian (because of its similarities to Spanish), but that English and French 'do not correspond to Spanish - neither in vocalization, or in the placement, or the colour, or the weight of words. ' He continued: 'It is not a question of interpretative equivalence: no, the sense can be right, but this correctness of translation, of meaning, can be the destruction of a poem. In many of the translations into French - I don 't say in all of them - my poetry escapes, nothing remains; one cannot protest because it says the same thing that one has written. But it is obvious that if I had been a French poet, I would not have said what I did in that poem, because the value of the words is so different. I would have written something else ' (Plimpton 1981, p. 63). With more 'pragmatic ' or less 'expressive ' writing, meanings are typically regarded as less dependent on the particular form of words used. In most pragmatic contexts, paraphrases or translations tend to be treated as less fundamentally problematic. However, even in such contexts, particular words or phrases which have an important function in the original language may be acknowledged to present special problems in translation. Even outside the humanities, academic texts concerned with the social sciences are a case in point.

The Whorfian perspective is in strong contrast to the extreme universalism of those who adopt the cloak theory. The Neo-Classical idea of language as simply the dress of thought is based on the assumption that the same thought can be expressed in a variety of ways. Universalists argue that we can say whatever we want to say in any language, and that whatever we say in one language can always be translated into another. This is the basis for the most common refutation of Whorfianism. 'The fact is, ' insists the philosopher Karl Popper, 'that even totally different languages are not untranslatable ' (Popper 1970, p. 56). The evasive use here of 'not untranslatable ' is ironic. Most universalists do acknowledge that translation may on occasions involve a certain amount of circumlocution.

Individuals who regard writing as fundamental to their sense of personal and professional identity may experience their written style as inseparable from this identity, and insofar as writers are 'attached to their words ', they may favour a Whorfian perspective. And it would be hardly surprising if individual stances towards Whorfianism were not influenced by allegiances to Romanticism or Classicism, or towards either the arts or the sciences. As I have pointed out, in the context of the written word, the 'untranslatability ' claim is generally regarded as strongest in the arts and weakest in the case of formal scientific papers (although rhetorical studies have increasingly blurred any clear distinctions). And within the literary domain, 'untranslatability ' was favoured by Romantic literary theorists, for whom the connotative, emotional or personal meanings of words were crucial (see Stone 1967, pp. 126-7, 132, 145).

Whilst few linguists would accept the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis in its 'strong ', extreme or deterministic form, many now accept a 'weak ', more moderate, or limited Whorfianism, namely that the ways in which we see the world may be influenced by the kind of language we use. Moderate Whorfianism differs from extreme Whorfianism in these ways:

the emphasis is on the potential for thinking to be 'influenced ' rather than unavoidably 'determined ' by language; it is a two-way process, so that 'the kind of language we use ' is also influenced by 'the way we see the world '; any influence is ascribed not to 'Language ' as such or to one language compared with another, but to the use within a language of one variety rather than another (typically a sociolect - the language used primarily by members of a particular social group); emphasis is given to the social context of language use rather than to purely linguistic considerations, such as the social pressure in particular contexts to use language in one way rather than another.
Of course, some polemicists still favour the notion of language as a strait-jacket or prison, but there is a broad academic consensus favouring moderate Whorfianism. Any linguistic influence is now generally considered to be related not primarily to the formal systemic structures of a language (langue to use de Saussure 's term) but to cultural conventions and individual styles of use (or parole). Meaning does not reside in a text but arises in its interpretation, and interpretation is shaped by sociocultural contexts. Conventions regarding what are considered appropriate uses of language in particular social contexts exist both in 'everyday ' uses of language and in specialist usage. In academia, there are general conventions as well as particular ones in each disciplinary and methodological context. In every subculture, the dominant conventions regarding appropriate usage tend to exert a conservative influence on the framing of phenomena. From the media theory perspective, the sociolects of sub-cultures and the idiolects of individuals represent a subtly selective view of the world: tending to support certain kinds of observations and interpretations and to restrict others. And this transformative power goes largely unnoticed, retreating to transparency.
Marshall McLuhan argued in books such as The Gutenberg Galaxy (1962) and Understanding Media (1964) that the use of new media was the prime cause of fundamental changes in society and the human psyche. The technological determinism of his stance can be seen as an application of extreme Whorfianism to the nature of media in general. Similarly, the extreme universalism of the cloak theorists has its media counterpart in the myth of technological neutrality (Winner 1977; Bowers 1988). My own approach involves exploring the applicability of moderate Whorfianism to the use of media.

References

Abrams, M. H. (1953): The Mirror and the Lamp: Romantic Theory and the Critical Tradition. Oxford: Oxford University Press
Bowers, C. A. (1988): The Cultural Dimensions of Educational Computing: Understanding the Non-Neutrality of Technology. New York: Teachers College Press
Bruner, J. S., J. S. Goodnow & G. A. Austin ([1956] 1962): A Study of Thinking. New York: Wiley
Fish, S. (1980): Is There a Text in This Class? The Authority of Interpretative Communities. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
McLuhan, M. (1962): The Gutenberg Galaxy: The Making of Typographic Man. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul
McLuhan, M. (1964): Understanding Media: The Extensions of Man. New York: McGraw-Hill
Plimpton, G. (ed.) (1963-1988): Writers at Work: The 'Paris Review ' Interviews, Vol. 5, 1981. London: Secker & Warburg/ Harmondsworth: Penguin (pagination differs)
Popper, K. (1970): 'Normal Science and its Dangers '. In I. Lakatos & A. Musgrave (eds.) (1970): Criticism and the Growth of Knowledge. London: Cambridge University Press
Sapir, E. (1929): 'The Status of Linguistics as a Science '. In E. Sapir (1958): Culture, Language and Personality (ed. D. G. Mandelbaum). Berkeley, CA: University of California Press
Steiner, G. (1975): After Babel: Aspects of Language and Translation. London: Oxford University Press
Stone, P. W. K. (1967): The Art of Poetry 1750-1820: Theories of Poetic Composition and Style in the Late Neo-Classic and Early Romantic Periods. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul
Whorf, B. L. (1940): 'Science and Linguistics ', Technology Review 42(6): 229-31, 247-8. Also in B. L. Whorf (1956): Language, Thought and Reality (ed. J. B. Carroll). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Winner, L. (1977): Autonomous Technology: Technics-Out-Of-Control as a Theme in Political Thought. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press

References: Abrams, M. H. (1953): The Mirror and the Lamp: Romantic Theory and the Critical Tradition. Oxford: Oxford University Press Bowers, C Bruner, J. S., J. S. Goodnow & G. A. Austin ([1956] 1962): A Study of Thinking. New York: Wiley Fish, S McLuhan, M. (1962): The Gutenberg Galaxy: The Making of Typographic Man. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul McLuhan, M Plimpton, G. (ed.) (1963-1988): Writers at Work: The 'Paris Review ' Interviews, Vol. 5, 1981. London: Secker & Warburg/ Harmondsworth: Penguin (pagination differs) Popper, K Sapir, E. (1929): 'The Status of Linguistics as a Science '. In E. Sapir (1958): Culture, Language and Personality (ed. D. G. Mandelbaum). Berkeley, CA: University of California Press Steiner, G Stone, P. W. K. (1967): The Art of Poetry 1750-1820: Theories of Poetic Composition and Style in the Late Neo-Classic and Early Romantic Periods. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul Whorf, B Winner, L. (1977): Autonomous Technology: Technics-Out-Of-Control as a Theme in Political Thought. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press

You May Also Find These Documents Helpful

  • Good Essays

    Donald M. Murray’s 1973 essay titled “The Maker’s Eye: Revising Your Own Manuscripts”, closely analyzes writers and their meticulous attention to detail present in all their works. He invites the reader into the world of an accomplished author and the mindset behind someone who has had a piece of their writing published for millions to see. The intended audience for this article is the aspiring student or author who’s interested in learning more about the world he may soon step into. Knowing this, Murray starts off the essay without any of the traditional expected fluff and instead brashly informs the reader of the difference between an expert author and one such as the reader.…

    • 372 Words
    • 2 Pages
    Good Essays
  • Good Essays

    This position is mainly associated with two American linguists, Edward Sapir (1884-1939) and Benjamin Lee Whorf (1897-1941). The Sapir-Whorf hypothesis as it is usually known, is that language actually determines thought. This theory is described as linguistic determinism.…

    • 2599 Words
    • 11 Pages
    Good Essays
  • Satisfactory Essays

    com week 1 quiz

    • 462 Words
    • 8 Pages

    A linguistic theory which states that language influences or shapes the speak­er’s ideas and views of the world is:…

    • 462 Words
    • 8 Pages
    Satisfactory Essays
  • Good Essays

    The differences between two people can make for an everlasting love because opposites can, and do, attract. In, “Love Poem” by John Frederick Nims, the poet describes this love. From her clumsiness to her calming influence, is a perfect description of his love for her strengths and weaknesses.…

    • 581 Words
    • 3 Pages
    Good Essays
  • Powerful Essays

    Their Eyes Were Watching God

    • 3170 Words
    • 13 Pages

    Vivas, Eliseo. “The Object of the Poem” Critical Theory since Plato. Ed. Hazard Adams. New York: Harcourt, 1971. 1069-77.…

    • 3170 Words
    • 13 Pages
    Powerful Essays
  • Powerful Essays

    A Shropshire Lad

    • 1275 Words
    • 6 Pages

    Scott-Kilvert, Ian. A.E. Housman: Writers and Their Work No. 69. Longmans, Green and Co., London. 1965.…

    • 1275 Words
    • 6 Pages
    Powerful Essays
  • Powerful Essays

    Cited: Coleridge, Samuel. English Romantic Poetry. Mineola, New York: Dover Publications, Inc., 1996. 102-04. Print.…

    • 1664 Words
    • 7 Pages
    Powerful Essays
  • Good Essays

    Anthropology 130

    • 734 Words
    • 3 Pages

    Understand Language Determinism (Sapir-Whorf Hypothesis) and Linguistic Relativity (language reflects, rather that determines, cultural reality).…

    • 734 Words
    • 3 Pages
    Good Essays
  • Powerful Essays

    Research Paper

    • 1185 Words
    • 5 Pages

    Cited: Irving, John. The World According to Garp. New York, NY: Pocket Books, 1979. Print.…

    • 1185 Words
    • 5 Pages
    Powerful Essays
  • Powerful Essays

    The Garifuna Language

    • 1072 Words
    • 5 Pages

    Hill, Jane H., P. J. Mistry, and Lyle Campbell. The Life of Language: Papers in Linguistics in Honor of William Bright. Berlin [etc.: Mouton De Gruyter, 1998. Print.…

    • 1072 Words
    • 5 Pages
    Powerful Essays
  • Good Essays

    If asked which cognitive ability you would miss the most if it were taken away, the majority of people would respond with the obvious choices of sight or hearing, but how many people would think about our sense of language? Language affects our lives in ways that we do not often realize. In the essay “How Language Shapes Thought” Lera Boroditsky argues that many of our cognitive abilities are enhanced, or hindered depending on the fundamental structure of our system of language. I found that Boroditsky used much of her own research in order to support her claims that direction, time and gender are concepts largely affected by the structural system of our language. Overall I found Boroditsky’s arguments to be sound and thorough. I agree with her claims that language shapes thought.…

    • 944 Words
    • 4 Pages
    Good Essays
  • Good Essays

    Sociology Definitions

    • 551 Words
    • 3 Pages

    Sapir-Whorf Hypothesis: A hypothesis that argues that the language a person uses determines his or her perception of reality.…

    • 551 Words
    • 3 Pages
    Good Essays
  • Better Essays

    language Paper

    • 1357 Words
    • 5 Pages

    Language is an important mechanism used in most individual’s everyday life. It helps define their culture, their backgrounds, who they are and where their place in the world is. Most of us don’t put a lot of thought into what mental processes work together to allow an individual to express his thoughts and ideas through language, but it is impossible to analyze the language development process without factoring in the important role that cognition plays in that event. This essay intends to present different hypothesis that try to explain how cognition and language are related, and how deeply connected they must be in order to allow the proper functioning and interaction of individuals in society. As thinkers try to understand if people that come from different backgrounds and speak different languages also think differently, or if there is a point of development that an individual has to reach of development from cognition required to acquire the language skills needed. The debate between psychologist about the relationship between language and cognition is one that is certain to be continues for ages to come (Harris, n.d.) .…

    • 1357 Words
    • 5 Pages
    Better Essays
  • Powerful Essays

    Edith Wharton

    • 1557 Words
    • 7 Pages

    "There are two ways of spreading light: to be the candle or the mirror that reflects it"…

    • 1557 Words
    • 7 Pages
    Powerful Essays
  • Powerful Essays

    Pinker's Dilemma

    • 10856 Words
    • 47 Pages

    2.1.1 Pinker’s account of how the mind works: the language of thought - a general…

    • 10856 Words
    • 47 Pages
    Powerful Essays