Behaviourism is primarily concerned with observable behaviour; the behaviour which can be watched and seen by others. It does not focus on any internal events, such as thinking, memory or the mind. It suggests that all behaviours are the result of some sort of stimulus, which triggers a response. Behaviourists believe that no matter how complicated the behaviour, it can be reduced down to a simple stimulus and response association. Watson described the purpose of psychology as “To predict, given the stimulus, what reaction will take place; or, given the reaction, state what the situation or stimulus is that has caused the reaction.” (1930, p11)
Behaviourism suggests that people have no personal agency and that the environment alone determines behaviour. We are assumed to be born Tabula Rasa and the behaviourism perspective believes we learn new behaviour through classical and operant conditioning. Watson believed that classical conditioning explained all aspects of psychology, that the way we behave is just patterns of stimulus and responses. It involves learning behaviour via association.
In the ethically dubious experiment Little Albert Watson and Raynor showed the way in which humans and animals could both learn by classical conditioning. Little Albert was a nine month old infant, Watson and Raynor used classical conditioning to make Little Albert afraid of a white rat which he previously had no fear of. Thus proving classical conditioning can be used to create a phobia. (Cited from Watson & Raynor 1920, pp. 1 – 14). “Give me a dozen healthy infants, well-formed, and my own specified world to bring them up in and I'll guarantee to take any one at random and train him to become any type of specialist I might select - doctor,
References: Andrew M. Colman, 2009. A Dictionary of Psychology (Oxford Dictionary of Psychology). 3 Edition. Oxford University Press, USA. Anthony Storr, 2001. Freud: A Very Short Introduction. Edition. Oxford University Press. Behaviorism. 2013. Behaviorism. [ONLINE] Available at: http://www.simplypsychology.org/behaviorism.html. [Accessed 29 January 2013]. Classics in the History of Psychology -- Watson (1913). 2013. Classics in the History of Psychology -- Watson (1913). [ONLINE] Available at: http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/Watson/views.htm. [Accessed 29 January 2013]. Erikson, E. H. (1950). Childhood and Society. New York: Norton. Freud, A Freud, S. (1896). Heredity and the etiology of the neuroses. In Standard edition (Vol. 3, pp. 142–156). Freud, S Jung, C. G., et al. (1964). Man and his Symbols, New York, N.Y.: Anchor Books, Doubleday Maslow, A Maslow, A. H. (1968). Toward a Psychology of Being (2nd Ed.). New York: D. Van Nostrand. Miller, G U (1967). Cognitive psychology. Appleton-Century-Crofts: New York Newell, A., & Simon, H Perspectives in Modern Psychology. 2013. Perspectives in Modern Psychology. [ONLINE] Available at: http://psychology.about.com/od/psychology101/a/perspectives.htm. [Accessed 29 January 2013]. Rogers, C. R. (1946). Significant Aspects of Client-Centered Therapy. American Psychologist 1, 415-422. Rogers, C. R. (1946). Significant Aspects of Client-Centered Therapy. American Psychologist 1, 415-422. Rogers, Carl. (1951). Client-centered Therapy: Its Current Practice, Implications and Theory. London: Constable. Simply Psychology - Articles for Students. 2013. Simply Psychology - Articles for Students. [ONLINE] Available at: http://www.simplypsychology.org. [Accessed 29 January 2013]. Tolman E. C. (1948). Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychological Review. 55, 189–208+--------------------- -+