Incorporating Hofstede 's Cultural Values Framework
Author(s): Bradley L. Kirkman, Kevin B. Lowe and Cristina B. Gibson
Source: Journal of International Business Studies, Vol. 37, No. 3 (May, 2006), pp. 285-320
Published by: Palgrave Macmillan Journals
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/3875261 .
Accessed: 01/10/2013 10:01
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at . http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp .
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.
.
Palgrave Macmillan Journals is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Journal of
International Business Studies.
http://www.jstor.org
This content downloaded from 130.253.4.14 on Tue, 1 Oct 2013 10:01:32 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
of
Business
Studies(2006)37, 285-320
Journal International
of
Business Allrightsreserved
0047-2506 $30.00
? 2006 Academy International www.jibs.net PERSPECTIVE
A
quarter
a
review
century of Hofstede 's
Culture 's
of
empirical cultural Bradley L. Kirkman ',
Kevin B. Lowe2 and
Cristina B. Gibson3
research
values
Consequences: incorporating framework
Abstract
of 'Department Management,
MaysBusiness
School,TexasA&MUniversity,
Station,
College of TX,USA;
2Department Business
Schoolof
Administration,
JosephM. Bryan
Business Economics, and of
University North
Carolina Greensboro, at North
Greensboro,
3Graduate
Schoolof
Carolina,
USA;
of
Management,
Irvine,
University California,
USA
California,
Correspondence:
Bradley L Kirkman, Department of
Management, Mays Business School, Texas
A&M University, 4221 TAMU, College
Station, TX 77843-4221, USA.
Tel: + 1 979 845 8813;
Fax: + 1 979 845 9641;
E-mail:brad.kirkman@tamu.edu
Received: 22 August 2002
Revised: 8 May 2005
Accepted: 17 May 2005
Online publication date: 4 May 2006
Since GeertHofstede 'sCulture 's
International
Differences Workin
Consequences:
have utilizedHofstede 's
Related
Values
(Sage, 1980) was published,researchers culturalvaluesframework a wide varietyof empiricalstudies.We review180 in and studies publishedin 40 businessand psychologyjournals two international annual volumes between 1980 and June 2002 to consolidate what is empiricallyverifiableabout Hofstede 'sculturalvalues framework.We discuss researchand make recommendationsfor limitationsin the Hofstede-inspired researchers who use Hofstede 'sframeworkin the future. of Studies(2006) 37, 285-320.
Business
Journal International doi:I0. I057/palgrave.jibs.8400202
cross-cultural cultural values; management Keywords:Hofstede;
Introduction
Research using a variety of frameworks has shown that national cultural values are related to workplace behaviors, attitudes and other organizational outcomes (e.g., Kluckhohn and Strodtbeck,
1961; Hall, 1976; Hofstede, 1980a; Trompenaars, 1993; Schwartz,
1994; Ronen and Shenkar, 1985). Perhaps the most influential of cultural classifications is that of Geert Hofstede. Over two decades have passed since the publication of Culture 's Consequences:
Values (Hofstede, 1980a),
InternationalDifferencesin Work-Related thousands of empirical studies; however, a compreheninspiring sive review of the impact of Hofstede 's frameworkis lacking. ' To fill this gap, we summarize and synthesize empirical research published between January 1980 and June 2002 that has applied
Hofstede 's framework to organizations. We focus on Hofstede 's framework rather than others, given evidence that it has had far greater impact (Sivakumar and Nakata, 2001). For example, the
Social Science Citations Index indicates that Hofstede 's work is more widely cited than others (cited 1,800 times through 1999;
Hofstede, 2001). Trompenaars (1993, iii), who has a competing framework, credits Hofstede 'for opening management 's eyes to the importance of the [cross-cultural management] subject '. Our purpose is both to summarize existing research and to direct and inform future research, rather than provide an in-depth discussion of Hofstede 's original study, a critique (e.g., Schwartz, 1994; Smith and Bond, 1999; McSweeney, 2002; Smith, 2002), or a replication
This content downloaded from 130.253.4.14 on Tue, 1 Oct 2013 10:01:32 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
286
A quarter century of Culture 'sConsequences
(e.g., Punnett and Withane (1990); Shackleton and
All, 1990; Merritt, 2000; Spector et al., 2001a).
We focus on aspects of Hofstede 's work not discussed in recent reviews and meta-analyses. For example, most researchers focused exclusively on individualism-collectivism (IND-COL) at the individual level of analysis (e.g., Triandis, 1995; Earley and Gibson, 1998; Oyserman et al., 2002).
Consequently, their implications and conclusions are based on a much narrower band of Hofstedeinspired research. Hofstede (2001) recently reviewed hundreds of studies published since his original book appeared in 1980. However, consistent with his opposition to applying the framework to the individual level, Hofstede 'ignores everything.., but the culture level comparisons ' (Smith,
2002, 123), thus missing an opportunity to draw conclusions across levels. Indeed, an analysis of references reveals very little overlap between studies we reviewed and those contained in previous reviews. Therefore our conclusions and implications should add value beyond previous reviews, as our purpose was to review studies at different levels of analysis and direction of effects to comprehensively integrate and synthesize the findings for all five cultural value dimensions in Hofstede 's framework. As we shall show, without such a comprehensive review, much of the Hofstede-inspired research has remained fragmented and in some cases redundant, and researchers are unable to benefit from the cumulative knowledge that accrues from an integrated body of quality research.
First, we provide a brief overview of Hofstede 's cultural value dimensions and how they were derived, and then we discuss our typology and the rationale for including/excluding articles. Next, we review findings, research accomplishments and challenges within each major domain of our typology. Finally, we provide direction for future
Hofstede-inspired research.
An overview of Hofstede 's cultural value dimensions Hofstede (1980a, 25) defined culture as 'the collective programming of the mind which distinguishes the members of one human group from another '.
His framework was developed using data from over
116,000 morale surveys from over 88,000 employees from 72 countries (reduced to 40 countries that
had more than 50 responses each) in 20 languages at IBM between 1967 and 1969 and again between
1971 and 1973. He later expanded the database with 10 additional countries and three regions (i.e.,
L et Bradley Kirkman al
Arab countries and East and West Africa). Based on a country level factor analysis, he classified the original 40 countries along four dimensions. The first is IND-COL,with IND defined as 'a loosely knit social framework in which people are supposed to take care of themselves and of their immediate families only ', while COL 'is characterized by a tight social framework in which people distinguish between ingroups and outgroups, they expect their ingroup to look after them, and in exchange for that they feel they owe absolute loyalty to it '(Hofstede, 1980b, 45). The second dimension is power distance (PD), defined as 'the extent to which a society accepts the fact that power in institutions and organizations is distributed unequally ' (1980b,
45). Third, uncertainty avoidance (UA) is defined as 'the extent to which a society feels threatened by uncertain and ambiguous situations and tries to avoid these situations by providing greater career stability, establishing more formal rules, not tolerating deviant ideas and behaviors, and believing in absolute truths and the attainment of expertise '
(1980b, 45). The fourth dimension is masculinity
(MAS)-femininity (FEM),with MASdefined as 'the extent to which the dominant values in society are
"masculine" - that is, assertiveness, the acquisition of money and things, and not caring for others, the quality of life, or people ' (1980b, 46) and
FEMdefined as the opposite of MAS.Michael Harris
Bond (Chinese Culture Connection, 1987) and later Hofstede and Bond (1988) developed a fifth dimension, Confucian dynamism (or long-term vs short-term orientation). Long-term orientation refers to future-oriented values such as persistence and thrift, whereas short-term orientation refers to past- and present-oriented values such as respect for tradition and fulfilling social obligations.
Hofstede 's (1980a) work has been criticized for: reducing culture to an overly simplistic four or five dimension conceptualization; limiting the sample to a single multinational corporation; failing to capture the malleability of culture over time; and ignoring within-country cultural heterogeneity
(Sivakumar and Nakata, 2001). In spite of criticism, researchers have favored this five-dimension framework because of its clarity, parsimony, and resonance with managers. Yet, even given the proliferation of studies incorporating the framework, there have been few attempts to summarize the empirical findings it has generated. Thus, a pressing and practical need in the literature at this time is a comprehensive review and discussion of ways to improve the use of Hofstede 's framework.
Journal of International Business Studies
This content downloaded from 130.253.4.14 on Tue, 1 Oct 2013 10:01:32 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
A quarter century of Culture 's Consequences
et
BradleyL Kirkman a!l
287
Criteria used to select articles and organize
Table 1 Journals searched, with corresponding number of articlesfound the review
We examine empirical research that assessed any
Number
Journalname of the five cultural values published in top-tier
22
management and applied psychology journals Academyof ManagementJournal
ScienceQuarterly
7
(Extejt and Smith, 1990; Gomez-Mejia and Balkin, Administrative
Advancesin GlobalLeadership
1
1992; Johnson and Podsakoff, 1994; Tahai and
Advancesin InternationalComparative
Management
1 and in journals specializing in interMeyer, 1999)
Annual Reviewof Psychology
0
national management and psychology (see Table 1).
British
0
Journalof Psychology
We excluded areas such as marketing or finance
British
journalof Social Psychology
2
because of the traditional delineation of these fields
European
1
Journalof Social Psychology
(in business schools and most academic journals)
Groupand OrganizationManagement
1
and the need to place limits on our comprehenHuman Relations
2
International siveness. We conducted article title, abstract, and
2
Journalof Commerceand Management
International
0
Sociology
Journalof Comparative methodology section searches from January 1980
International
1
Journalof ConflictManagement to June 2002. We first conducted computer-assisted
International
Relations
5
Journalof Intercultural searches (e.g., ABI-Inform, Ebsco Host Academic
International
2
Journalof OrganizationalAnalysis
Full-Text Elite) when full text was available; otherInternational
2
Journalof Psychology wise, each journal was physically searched. We
International
Studiesof Managementand Organization
1
then conducted computer-assisted keyword Journalof AppliedBehavioralScience
1
searches within journals using variations on HofJournalof AppliedPsychology
7
stede 's terms (e.g., COL, collectivists, collectivistic).
Journalof AppliedSocial Psychology
3
We included articles only if the authors empirically Journalof Cross-Cultural
16
Psychology
Social Psychology
1
assessed the cultural values using either primary or Journalof Experimental
BusinessStudies
Journalof International
41
data. Primarydata include research that secondary 5 assessed values through surveys, experiments, or Journalof InternationalManagement
Journalof Management
9
other direct methods. Secondary data include
0
Journalof Management Studies research that used Hofstede 's country scores to,
Journalof OrganizationalBehavior
6
for example, create cultural distance measures Journalof Personality
0
and Social Psychology
(Kogut and Singh, 1988). We also included a few
Journalof Personality
11
studies that used country scores to assign cultural Journalof Researchin Personality
1
Journalof WorldBusiness values to individuals (e.g., Bochner and Hesketh,
1
1
Quarterly
Leadership
1994; Palich et al., 1995). A detailed analysis of the
Review
6
Management International shortcomings of this particular method appears in
1
Management Science our discussion. We mention these limitations here
MultinationalBusinessReview
2 to warn readersabout the variance in the quality of
0
OrganizationScience methodologies used in the studies we review.
4
OrganizationalBehaviorand HumanDecisionProcesses
Previous reviews of the IND-COL literature have
PersonnelPsychology
2 discussed the difficulty of comparing studies
Bulletin
0
Psychological
that used a wide variety of different measures
Review
0
Psychological
Small GroupResearch and methods to operationalize cultural values
1
11
StrategicManagementJournal
(Earley and Gibson, 1998; Oyserman et al., 2002).
Thus, comparing means directly across studies may
Total
180
be problematic. Space prohibits a critique of methodology for each of the studies, but readers should note that authors took different degrees of care to control for methodological concerns such as response bias or halo effects. Thus, as we note throughout, caution should be exercised in interpreting the findings.2
To organize the review, we used a two-tier classification scheme. The first tier pertained to the role of cultural values in the relationships
investigated. Research on culture has examined main associations between values and outcomes, as well as cultural values as moderators.Main effect studies have been labeled 'Type I ' and moderator studies 'Type II ' (Lytle et al., 1995). Our second classification was by level of analysis, whether individual, group/organizational, or country. In
Journal of International Business Studies
This content downloaded from 130.253.4.14 on Tue, 1 Oct 2013 10:01:32 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
A quarter century of Culture 'sConsequences
288
spite of Hofstede 's (1980a, 2001) argument against using his dimensions for purposes other than country level studies, we found that a majority of researchers had adapted them for use at the individual or group/organization levels. All of these levels are valid, depending on the research question and on whether there is more commonality within, than between groups (Sivakumar and Nakata,
2001). Thus, we argue that excluding these studies would create an incomplete picture of Hofstede 's impact. In addition, analyses of the same data at different levels of analysis do not necessarily lead to equivalent findings or replication of dimensions
(Leung and Bond, 1989; Hofstede et al., 1993,
2001). To accurately sort the studies into the proper level of analysis, we scrutinized each study 's theory and method section to determine the level at which the study was conceptualized and analyzed, respectively. The level at which the data were collected was not always as informative. For example, studies using individual level survey data could be classified as individual, group/organizational, or country level. For individual level studies, data must have been collected and analyzed at the individual level of analysis and tied to individual level outcomes. For group/organizational, the data had to be aggregated to the group or organizational level and linked to group/organizational outcomes. For country level studies, either individual level data had to be aggregatedby country and linked to country level outcomes or pre-existing country level measures (such as Hofstede 's, 1980a country level scores) had to be used. Table 2 depicts our 2 x 3 classification with the corresponding number of articles reviewed within each of the six cells. To ease interpretation, we further organize findings by topic using subject matter headings from business and psychology. To facilitate reader comparisons of topics at respective levels,
Table 3 shows the topics on the vertical axis and the level of analysis on the horizontal. Also, Supplementary Appendices A and B contain detailed article summaries on culture as main and moderator effects, respectively, for all 180 articles in our review.3
et
LKirkmana
Bradley
Research incorporating Hofstede 's cultural value dimensions
We first review culture as a main effect (i.e., Type I studies) at the individual level of analysis, and then at the group/organization and country levels. We then review findings that incorporate culture as a moderator (i.e., Type II studies) at these levels of analysis. When a study included multiple levels or type of effect, we discuss each finding in the appropriate section and note cross-listings.
Type I studies of culture at the individual level of analysis
Our review uncovered two basic types of Type I individual level study: cross-cultural and monocultural. In both types of study, researchers typically examine relationships between individuals ' cultural values and various outcomes; however, in cross-cultural studies two or more countries are normally included, whereas in mono-cultural studies all individuals emanate from the same country.
Researchers have shown that there is plenty of within-country variation on cultural values
(Hofstede, 1980a; Au, 1999). Clearly, 'people vary on pivotal psychological dimensions (e.g., PD beliefs, traditionality) both on a between-country basis and on a within-country basis ' (Brockner,
2005: 355). Thus, even though mono-cultural studies may not be viewed as technically crosscultural, we still include those studies empirically assessing cultural values in only a single country because of the theoretical importance of understanding within country cultural variation.
Cultural values were associated with outcomes in management and applied psychology domains, including: change management; conflict management; decision-making; human resource management (HRM);leadership; organizational citizenship behavior (OCB);work-relatedattitudes; negotiation behavior; reward allocation; and individual behavior relating to group processes and personality.
Change management (4)3
Collectivists in the US showed more positive group attitudes toward a new technology and had better
scheme used for literaturereview and number of articlesincluded
Table 2 Classification level Individual
Countrylevel
Total
64
23
87
Culture as a main effect
Culture as a moderator
Total
level
Group/organization
6
5
11
78
4
82
148
32
180
Note:Ifa studywas listedin morethanone section,it was countedonly once in the sectionin whichit firstappeared.
Journal of International Business Studies
This content downloaded from 130.253.4.14 on Tue, 1 Oct 2013 10:01:32 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
A quarter century of Culture 'sConsequences
et
L
Bradley Kirkmanal
289
Table 3 Researchsubject matter by level of analysis
Individual
Group/organization
Country
Elron (1997)main,
Oeztzel (1998)main
Smith et al. (1998)main
Change management James (1993)main,Geletkanycz
(1997)main, Anakwe et al.
(1999)main, Eby et al. (2000)main
Conflict management
Leung (1987)main, Leung
(1 988)main, Cocroft and
Ting-Toomey (1994)main
Gabrielidis et al. (1997)main
Decision-making
Ali (1993)main, Nooteboom et al.
(1997)main, Mitchell et al.
(2000)main, Steensma et al.
(2000a)main
management
Ozawa et al. (1996)main, Cable and judge (1994)main, Ramamoorthy
Newman and Nollen (1996)main
Roth and O 'Donnell (1996)main,
and Carroll(1998)main,
Human resource
Schuler and Rogovsky (1998)main,
(1986)main,
Leadership
Earley
Casimir and Keats (1996)main,
Pillaiand Meindl (1998)main,
Helgstrand and Stuhlmacher
(1999)main, Jung and Avolio
(1999)mod,
Ryan et al. (1999)main
Earley et al. (1999)main
Pillai and Meindl (1998)main
Shenkar and Zeira (1992)main
Offerman and Hellmann
(1997)main, House et al. (1999)main
Chan and
Drasgow (2001)main
OCB
Moorman and Blakely (1995)man,
Van Dyne et al. (2000)main
Work-relatedattitudes Bochner and Hesketh (1994)main,
Palich et al. (1995)mod, Bennett
(1999)main, Chiu (1999)main,
Clugston et al. (2000)main, Feldman and Bolino (2000)main, Lee et al.
(2000a)main, Lee et al. (2000b)mod,
Martella and Maass (2000)mod,
Schaubroeck et al. (2000)mod,
Vandenberghe et al. (2001)mod,
Harpaz et al. (2002)main,
Schneider and DeMeyer
Weber et al.
(1996)main, Peterson et al.
(1995)main, Van de Vliert and Van
(1991)main,
Yperen (1996)main, Peterson and
Smith (1997)main, Robie et al.
Gong et al. (2001)main,
(1998)mod,
Spector et al. (2001 b)main, Spector et al. (2002)mod
Thomas and
Au (2002)main ' mod
Negotiation
Arunachalam et al. (1998)main,
Tinsley and Pillutla (1998)man,
Brett and Okumura (1998)man,
Pearson and Stephan (1998)main
Morris et al. (1998)main, Gelfand and Realo (1999)mod, Probst et al.
(1999)main, Ng and Van Dyne
(2001)mod, Tinsley (2001)main
Tinsley and Brett (2001)main,
Wade-Benzoni et al. (2002)main
Studies
Business
journalof International
This content downloaded from 130.253.4.14 on Tue, 1 Oct 2013 10:01:32 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
290
"Aquarter century of Culture 's Consequences
et
BradleyLKirkman al
Table 3 Continued
Individual
Reward allocation
Group/organization
Country
Mann et al. (1985)ma" ', Leung and
Iwawaki (1988)main, Hui et al.
(1991)main, Tower et al. (1997)main
Chen et al. (1997)main, C.C. Chen
et al. (1998a, b)main,
McLean-Parks
et al. (1999)main,
Gomez et al.
(2000)main
Behavior relating to
group processes and personality Triandis et al. (1988)main, Earley
(1989)mod,
Hui and Villareal
Earley (1993)mod,
Oyserman (1993)main, Wagner
(1995)mod, Yamaguchi et al.
(1995)main, Eby and Dobbins
(1997)main, Kwan et al.
(1997)main, Wojciszke (1997)main,
Chen et al. (1998a, b)mod,
Oyserman et al. (1998)main, Grimm et al. (1999)main, Tafarodi et al.
(1999)main, Thomas
Kirkman and Shapiro(1999)main,
(2000)main,
Satterwhite et al.
(2000)main,
(1989)main,
Chatman and Barsade (1995)mod,
Chatman et al. (1998)mod,
Cox et al. (1991)main
Erez and Somech (1996)mod
Eby and Dobbins (1997)main
Earley (1999)mod
Gibson (1999)mod
Kirkman and Shapiro (2001 b)main
Lam et al. (2002a)mod
Krug and Nigh (1998)main
Oyserman et al.
(2002)main
Carpenter and Radhakrishnan
Gibson and ZellmerBruhn (2001)main, Kirkman and
Shapiro (2001 a)main
(2000)main,
Entrepreneurship
Social networks
Morriset al. (1993)main,
Morris et al. (1994)main
Thomas and Mueller (2000)main
Zaheer and Zaheer
Manev and Stevenson (2001)main
(1997)main,
Entrymodes
Kogut and Singh (1988)main
Erramilli (1991)main,
Kim and
Hwang (1992)main, Shane
(1992)main, Shane (1994)main
Erramilli (1996)main
Padmanabhan and Cho
(1996)main, Pan (1996)main
Pan and Tse (1996)main
Anand and Delios (1997)main
Erramilli et al. (1997)main
Barkemaand Vermeulen
(1998)main, Hennart and Larimo
(1998)main, Pan and Tse
(2000)main, Arora and Fosfuri
(2000)main, Brouthers and
Brouthers(2000)main, Brouthers and Brouthers (2001)main, Chang and Rosenzweig (2001)main
Harzing (2002)main, Erramilli et al.
(2002)mod, Pan (2002)main,
Nachum (2003)main
Journal of International Business Studies
This content downloaded from 130.253.4.14 on Tue, 1 Oct 2013 10:01:32 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
A quarter century of Culture 's Consequences
et
BradleyL Kirkman al
291
Table 3 Continued
Individual
Group/organization
Country
Foreign direct
Benito and Gripsrud(1992)main,
investment
Li and Guisinger (1992)main
Loree and Guisinger (1995)main,
Kallunki et al. (2001)main, Thomas
and Grosse (2001)main,Habib and
Zurawicki (2002)main
Liand Guisinger (1991)main, Datta and Puia (1995)main, Barkema et al.
(1996)main, Barkema et al.
(1997)main, Barkema and
Vermeulen (1997)main
Jointventure characteristics and performance Gomez-Mejia and Palich
(1997)main, Park and Ungson
(1997)main, Morosini et al.
(1998)main, Glaister and Buckley
(1999)main, Luo and Peng
(1999)main, Merchant and
Schendel (2000)main, Hakanson and Nobel (2001)main, Luo
(2001 a)main, Luo (2001 b)man,
Luo and Park (2001)main, Luo et al.
(2001)mod,
Reuer(2001)main
Luo (2002)mann, Pothukuchi et al.
(2002)main
Alliance formation
Dickson and Weaver (1997)mod
Innovation and
Kashlak et al. (1998)main
Steensma et al. (2000b)mainmod
Shane (1995)main, Shane et al.
(1995)main, Jones and Teegen
(2001)main, Richards and De Carolis
research and development (2003)main
Societal outcomes
Frankeet al. (1991)main,Diener and
(e.g., wealth, national accounting systems,
Diener (1995)mainmod, Diener et al.
(1995)main, Diener et al. (2000)mod
number of intellectual property violations
Salter and Niswander (1995)main
Riahi-Belkaoui
(1998)main,Husted
(1999)main, Ronkainen and
Guerrero-Cusumano (2001)main
Motivation
Erez and Earley (1987)mod,
Dorfman and Howell (1988)mod,
Earley (1994)mod, Eylon and Au
Lam et al. (2002a)mod
(1999)mod,
Organizational justice
Lind et al. (1997)mod, Au et al.
Brockner et al.
Brockner et al.
(2000)mod,
Lam et al. (2002b)mod
(2001)mod,
(2002)mod,
main, main effect study; mod, moderating effect study.
retention when a technology presentation contained a group-relevance theme (the same was true for individualists when the presentation contained a strong self-relevance theme) (James, 1993). COL was negatively related to receptivity to distance
learning technology (Anakwe et al., 1999), and was positively related to perceptions of an organization 's readiness to change to team-based selling
(Eby et al., 2000) in the US. Using Hofstede 's
(1980a) country scores to assign cultural values to
Studies
Business
journalof International
This content downloaded from 130.253.4.14 on Tue, 1 Oct 2013 10:01:32 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
292
SA quarter century of Culture 'sConsequences
managers in 20 countries, Geletkanycz (1997) found that managers were more likely to believe that the future chief executive officer (CEO) of their company should have the same expertise as the current CEO, and that the company 's future corporate strategy should be the same as the existing corporate strategy (i.e., essentially demonstrating acceptance of the status quo) when they were high on IND, and low on UA, PD, and longterm orientation.
Conflictmanagement (4)
Hong Kong (HK) subjects (who were significantly higher in COL)preferredbargaining and mediation more than US subjects in a scenario-based experiment with undergraduate and non-student subjects
(Leung, 1987). In both countries, subjects ' procedure preferences were related to the extent to which the procedure was perceived as favorable to the disputants, fair, capable of animosity reduction, and allowing process control. Leung (1988) found that HK subjects were more likely to sue a stranger than were US subjects; and COLwas associated with higher likelihood of suing when the dispute was between strangers (but not friends). US students
(who scored significantly lower on both IND and
COL; see our discussion section for debate on
IND-COL as unipolar or bipolar at the individual level) used more antisocial, self-attribution, hint, and self-presentation strategies than Japanese subjects, who used more indirect face strategies
(Cocroft and Ting-Toomey, 1994). Finally, Mexican student subjects (who were significantly higher on
COL and IND and lower on FEMthan US subjects) showed more concern for others ' outcomes in conflict resolution than US students (Gabrielidis et al., 1997). More specifically, Mexican scores on accommodation and collaboration were higher than those in the US.
Decision-making(4)
IND was positively related to consultative, participative, and autocratic decision-making styles and to attitudes toward risk in Saudi Arabian managers
(Ali, 1993). Nooteboom et al. (1997) found that UA was negatively related to the probability of a potential loss by transaction partners, but unrelated to the size of loss among 97 firm-supplier alliances in the Netherlands. Mitchell et al. (2000), using
Hofstede 's (1980a) country scores at the individual level and assessing business professionals ' cognitive scripts, found that IND and PD were positively related to 'ability ' scripts (i.e., given resources, the
et
L
Bradley Kirkmanal
person is able to carry out a goal); IND was positively related to 'willingness ' scripts (i.e., given resources, the person will want to carry out a goal); and the relationship between 'arrangements 'scripts
(i.e., access to required materials) and starting a new business was stronger for individualists than for collectivists in the US, Mexico, China, Japan, and Chile. Using country scores, entrepreneurs from seven countries (Australia, Finland, Greece,
Indonesia, Mexico, Norway, and Sweden) viewed cooperative strategies with other firms as more acceptable when they were lower in IND and MAS, but higher in UA (Steensma et al., 2000a). Those higher, rather than lower, in IND and UA preferred contractual safeguardsmore, and perceived partner commonality was positively linked to UA.
HRM
(5)
US undergraduates, who were significantly higher on IND than Japanese (using Singelis et al. 's (1995) self-construal measure), perceived a change to an affirmative action program less favorably and fair than Japanese subjects (Ozawa et al., 1996). COL was negatively related to preferences for individualbased pay in the US (Cable and Judge, 1994) and to the use of selection tests, formal appraisalpractices, and desire for promotions based on merit, and positively related to preference for equality-based rewards and employment security among US business undergraduates (Ramamoorthy and Carroll,
1998). Both COL and PD interacted with feedback type such that US subjects ' performance increased as a result of both positive and negative feedback, but in England (where COL and IND were significantly higher), only positive feedback resulted in performance increases (Earley,1986). In a second sample, the importance of feedback received and trust in supervisor partially mediated the main effects of praise, criticism, and culture on performance. In a sample of managers completing a managerial simulation in the US, Czech Republic and PRC (US subjects were significantly higher on
IND than either Czech or PRCsubjects who did not differ), Earley et al. (1999) found that individual feedback played a role for both individualists and collectivists, whereas group feedback was critical only for collectivists.
Leadership (4)
In a sample of Anglo- and Chinese-Australian managers, Casimir and Keats (1996) assessed preferences for leadership styles from among four choices (i.e., created by crossing the extent to
Journal of International Business Studies
This content downloaded from 130.253.4.14 on Tue, 1 Oct 2013 10:01:32 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
A quarter century of Culture 'sConsequences
which a leader is high or low on both concern for group performance and maintenance of group relations). Both cultures preferred leaders who expressed high concern for both performance and group relations, and did not differ significantly on
IND-COL. In high-stress work environments, both cultures maintained their preference for highconcern leaders; however, in low-stress environments, Chinese respondents preferreda leader who showed concern for group relations equally to leaders high on both types of concern. In a second study, COL was positively related to the level of charismatic leadership, which in turn was positively related to supervisory ratings of work unit performance, job satisfaction, satisfaction with the leader, and leader effectiveness in over 100 work units of a US firm (Pillai and Meindl, 1998).
Helgstrand and Stuhlmacher (1999) found that both Danish and US high school and undergraduate students rated leaders who were feminine and individualistic as most effective (Danes were lower on PD). Finally, using horizontal (H) and vertical
(V) aspects of IND and COL (i.e., VCOLrepresents a tendency to view the self as an aspect of the group, see members of an ingroup as different from the self, and accept inequality; HCOL represents the tendency to view the self as an aspect of the group, see members of an ingroup as similarto the self, and value equality;VIND represents the extent to which an individual 's self-concept is autonomous but expects inequality;and HIND represents the extent to which an individual 's self-concept is autonomous and the individual is seen as equal to others),
Chan and Drasgow (2001) surveyed military recruits and junior college students in Singapore and the US to explore links between cultural values and various dimensions of motivation to lead (MTL).They found that HIND was negatively related to noncalculative MTL (i.e., people lead only when they are not calculative about the costs of leading relative to the benefits) and socialnormative MTL (i.e., people lead because they feel a sense of responsibility or duty); and VIND was positively related to affective identity MTL (i.e., some people just like to lead others) and socialnormative MTL, and negatively related to noncalculative MTL.
OCB
(2)
In a sample of US financial services employees, COL
(assessed using the values, norms, and beliefs dimensions of Wagner and Moch (1986)) was positively related to several dimensions of OCB
et
L
Bradley Kirkmanal
293
after controlling for procedural justice (Moorman and Blakely, 1995). In a sample of cooperative housing residents in the US, COL was positively related to OCB,with organization-based self-esteem fully mediating the relationship (Van Dyne et al.,
2000). attitudes (8)
Work-related
Bochner and Hesketh (1994) surveyed Australian bank employees representing 28 different nationalities, assigned each a country score for IND-COL and PD, and placed them into high and low groups on the two values. Collectivists reported having more informal contact with fellow workers, knew staff better, and were more likely to work on a team than alone compared with individualists. Those high, rather than low, in PD were less open with their superiors, had more contact with them, described their supervision as being more close and direct, were more task-oriented, and had greater beliefs in Theory X (i.e., a management style favoring centralized decision-making, tight control, and hierarchy). Bennett (1999), after first confirming that a US sample was significantly lower on COL and higher on MAS than a PRC sample, found that COL was positively related to favorable attitudes towards group activities and cooperation in both countries. MAS was negatively related to attitudes towards human development, but only in the US sample.
Chiu (1999) found that individualists scored higher on positive affect and job satisfaction and lower on work strain than collectivists in a sample of Singaporean and HK nurses, who were significantly higher on COL and lower on IND than
Australian and US nurses. Clugston et al. (2000) assessed the relationships among Hofstede 's four original cultural values and three bases (i.e., affective, continuance, and normative) and foci
(i.e., organization, supervisor, and workgroup) of commitment using surveys in a US public agency. COL was positively related to affective commitment to supervisors and the workgroup, continuance commitment to the workgroup, and normative commitment to all foci; and PD was positively related to affective commitment to the organization and both continuance and normative commitment to all foci. Lee et al. (2000a) found that individualists were more attuned toward a promotion focus (i.e., the pursuit of gains and aspiration toward ideals), whereas collectivists were more attuned toward a prevention focus (i.e., the avoidance of losses, and the fulfillment of obliga-
Journal of International Business Studies
This content downloaded from 130.253.4.14 on Tue, 1 Oct 2013 10:01:32 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
294
A quarter century of Culture 's Consequences
tions) in a sample of US undergraduate subjects who were significantly higher on IND (i.e., independent self-construal) and lower on COL (i.e., interdependent self-construal) than HK undergraduates. Harpaz et al. (2002) found that, over time, new work entrants from low UA countries (based on Hofstede 's (1980a) country scores assigned to individuals) had increased work centrality. In a study of behavioral responses to job dissatisfaction in a sample of executive education participants in
New Zealand and HK (New Zealanders were higher in HIND and VCOLthan those in HK),Thomas and
Au (2002) found that HIND was positively related to voice and that VCOL was positively related to neglect and negatively related to loyalty. Finally, cultural distance (based on the degree of difference between an individual and others in their context using country scores) was unrelated to opportunities to learn or further develop skills, internship satisfaction, or organizational commitment among a sample of US MBA students in overseas internships (Feldman and Bolino, 2000).
L et Bradley Kirkman al
Negotiation (9)
HK negotiators (who scored significantly higher on
COL and lower on IND than US negotiators) obtained higher joint outcomes than those in the
US in a two-party negotiation experiment (Arunachalam et al., 1998). Negotiators with a high, rather than a low, best alternative to a negotiated agreement (BATNA)obtained larger outcomes in both
HK and the US. Mediation was associated with higher joint outcomes and had a stronger effect in the US than in HK. In a similar experiment, Tinsley and Pillutla (1998) found that US subjects scored significantly higher on self-enhancement (i.e., a cultural value 'conceptually similar ' to IND; p 713) and lower on self-transcendence (i.e., a cultural value conceptually similar to COL) than HK subjects using Schwartz 's (1992) cultural value inventory. US subjects rated self-interested and joint problem-solving as more appropriatethan their HK counterparts. Further, cooperative instructions
listic but less hierarchical (i.e., lower in PD) than the Japanese, found that individualists endorsed self-interest in negotiations, and that negotiators with stronger hierarchical values endorsed distributive tactics and reported spending significantly more time discussing power. Brazilians(who scored significantly lower on IND than US subjects) preferred accommodation, collaboration, and withdrawal negotiating styles more, whereas US subjects preferred competition in student dyad negotiation experiments (Pearson and Stephan,
1998). US subjects preferred negotiation styles reflecting a high concern for self, whereas Brazilians preferred a style reflecting a high concern for others. Finally, Brazilians made accommodations and avoided conflict more when the conflict was with an ingroup, rather than an outgroup, member, whereas US subjects treated ingroup and outgroup members similarly. Importantly, results were replicated using IND-COL rather than country.
In a sample drawn from the US, the People 's
Republic of China (PRC),India, and the Philippines
(with culture assessed using Schwartz 's(1994) value inventory), Chinese managers preferred an avoiding style, with societal conservatism (i.e., low openness to change) fully mediating the effects of country on conflict style; and US managers preferred a competing style of conflict, with selfenhancement (i.e., IND) fully mediating the effects of country on conflict style (Morriset al., 1998). In a sample of US undergraduates, Probst et al. (1999) found type of prisoner 's dilemma (i.e., single-group vs intergroup) moderated the main effects that
VIND and VCOL had with cooperation such that vertical individualists were least cooperative in the single-group prisoner 's dilemma but were more cooperative in the intergroup dilemma (when cooperation with the group maximized personal outcomes). Vertical collectivists were most cooperative in the single-group dilemma but less in the intergroup dilemma (when group defection resulted in maximum group outcomes). Examining businesspeople from Japan, Germany, and the US,
were interpreted by US subjects as meaning they should strive for joint gain, whereas HK subjects interpreted them as meaning they should strive for equality. Finally, dyad sum was positively related to satisfaction for US (but not HK) subjects, whereas dyad difference was positively related to satisfaction for HK (but not US) subjects.
In a series of inter- and intra-cultural dyad experiments, Brett and Okumura (1998), after showing that US negotiators were more individua-
Tinsley (2001) showed that IND was positively related to using interest strategies and negatively related to using power strategies (Japanese respondents were significantly lower on IND than German or US respondents). Studying business students in
HK and the US, Tinsley and Brett (2001) found that
US students were more self-directed, less hierarchical, and less tradition-bound than HK students; US students placed greater emphasis on discussing interests and synthesizing multiple interests than
Journal of International Business Studies
This content downloaded from 130.253.4.14 on Tue, 1 Oct 2013 10:01:32 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
A quarter century of Culture 's Consequences
did HK students; and American dyads were significantly more likely to reach an integrative outcome than were HK dyads. Finally, Wade-Benzoni et al. (2002) found that US MBAs (who were significantly higher on IND than Japanese undergraduates) were less cooperative, reached fewer equal solutions, and expected others to be less cooperative than the Japanese.
Reward allocation (8)
Assessing COLin both Japan and the US, Leung and
Iwawaki (1988) found that the more collectivistic a subject, the more he or she followed the equality rule (everyone receives the same reward) and the less he or she followed the equity rule (rewards allocated based on effort). Contrary to expectations, Japanese and US subjects did not significantly differ on COL. After including South Korea in the dataset, for low input subjects (who participated infrequently), there were no differences across countries in the use of the equity norm, nor did they allocate more equally with friends (i.e., ingroups) and more equitably with strangers (i.e., outgroups). Generous allocators (i.e., low-input allocators using equity rules and high-input allocators using equality rules) were better liked (in all three countries) and rated as fairer than allocators who were less generous (findings held in Japan and the US, but not in South Korea). Hui et al. (1991) found that compared with US subjects, when resources were plentiful, HK subjects (who scored significantly higher on COL)put a greateremphasis on equal allocation of rewards; when the reward was fixed, HK subjects were more generous and treated close friends, compared with co-workers, more generously, whereas US subjects made no such distinction.
Primary school Japanese subjects (who scored significantly higher on COL) were more likely to follow 'equal-say ' rules than Australian subjects, who tended to follow more 'self-interest ' rules in a game experiment (Mann et al., 1985). In a study of
British and Russian students (Tower et al., 1997), when British students (who scored significantly lower on COL) were low performers or allocating rewards with a co-worker who was a friend, they allocated more to themselves; and there were no allocation differences for the British when performance was high or when the co-worker was a stranger. In contrast, when Russian students were high performers or allocating rewards to a coworker who was a stranger, they allocated more reward to themselves; and there were no allocation
et
BradleyLKirkman al
295
differences for the Russians when performance was low or the co-worker was a friend.
VCOLwas positively related to reward allocation reform in an in-basket exercise of managers in the
PRC (Chen et al., 1997). In contrast, HCOL was marginally and negatively related to reward allocation reform. An interaction effect for VCOL and
HCOLindicated that the negative relation between
HCOLand support for reform holds only under the condition of low VCOL.VCOLwas also negatively related to egalitarian allocation preferences, whereas HCOLwas negatively related to differential allocation preferences. In a second study, Chen et al. (1998a) asked undergraduate students in HK and the US to make reward allocation decisions after reading a case-vignette. Rewardallocation was more differential when task interdependence was low rather than high, and when the goal was productivity or fairness rather than solidarity in both countries. In the high interdependence situation, achievement motivation was negatively related to the differential allocation, but when it was low, achievement motivation was negatively related to differential allocation only for the HK subjects, but positively related for US subjects. COL was negatively related to differential allocation in
HKbut not the US. Individualistic MBAstudents in
Singapore and the US took slightly more time to recover, and less time to allocate, resources than collectivists (McLean-Parkset al., 1999). In both cultures, equity rules were used more when distributing a resource, and equality when recovering resources. After showing that Mexican MBAs were significantly more collectivistic than US students,
Gomez et al. (2000) found that collectivists gave more generous evaluations to ingroup, rather than outgroup, members in a teamwork scenario-based experiment. Individualists valued task inputs in determining evaluations more so than did collectivists, whereas both valued equity-based rewards.
Individualbehavior relating to group processes and personality (16)
As some of these studies are only tangentially related to organizational contexts and are more social psychological in focus (e.g., Hui and Villareal, 1989; Oyserman, 1993; Yamaguchi et al., 1995;
Kwan et al., 1997; Wojciszke, 1997; Oyserman et al.,
1998; Grimm et al., 1999; Tafarodi et al., 1999;
Carpenter and Radhakrishnan, 2000; Satterwhite et al., 2000), we highlight only those that have clear links to work-related outcomes. For example,
Triandis et al. (1988) conducted three studies to
Journal of International Business Studies
This content downloaded from 130.253.4.14 on Tue, 1 Oct 2013 10:01:32 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
296
A quarter century of Culture 'sConsequences
examine the relationship between IND and COL and self-ingroup relations. Surveys responses from
US undergraduates were factor-analyzed, and a multifaceted conceptualization of IND emerged.
US respondents viewed competition as occurring between individuals, whereas Puerto Rican respondents viewed competition as occurring more between ingroups and outgroups, rather than within ingroups.
COL was positively related to positive assessments of group processes in a study of undergraduates from 14 countries studying in New
Zealand (Thomas, 1999). Cultural distance on
COL (i.e., how different, on average, each individual is from other group members) was negatively related to group receptiveness. In a sample of US undergraduates, COL was positively related to selfefficacy for teamwork, need for social approval, and positive past experience working in teams (Eby and
Dobbins, 1997). In support of their theoretical model (Kirkmanand Shapiro, 1997), Kirkman and
Shapiro (2000) found that COL was positively related to receptivity to team-based rewards using employee surveys in a US insurance company.
Using surveys from self-managing work team
(SMWT)members in Belgium, Finland, the Philippines and the US, COLwas also positively related to team members ' job satisfaction and organizational commitment; and resistance to teams mediated the relationships between COL and both satisfaction and commitment, whereas resistance to self-management partially mediated the negative relationship between PD and commitment (Kirkman and
Shapiro, 2001a). Gibson and Zellmer-Bruhn (2001) investigated teamwork metaphor use in interviews in France, Puerto Rico, the Philippines and the US in five multinational firms. PD was negatively related to using metaphors containing clear role content (e.g., family or military metaphors), and
IND was negatively related to metaphors broad in scope (e.g., community metaphor).
Researchaccomplishments
Whereas Hofstede (1980a) was clear that his conceptualization and operationalization of cultural values was intended only for the country level, researchers have liberally adapted them for Type I studies at the individual level. Adaptation in this manner has both strengths and weaknesses. On the positive side, it has provided a new way to consider, describe and measure culture (Bond, 2002). Study variance that might have been attributed to other variables or explained with post hoc analyses has
et
L
Bradley Kirkmanal
been used to derive and test theories about the cultural antecedents to individual outcomes in organizations. The framework demonstrates multimethod utility at the individual level of analysis, with Type I studies utilizing a variety of methods including experimental, managerial simulation, survey research, in-basket exercises, and scenarios.
On the downside, there are clearly disconnects between the theoretical and methodological underpinning of Hofstede 's (1980a) conceptualization and that of researchers working at the individual level. For example, findings that are incongruent with Hofstede (1980a), such as the studies that have shown that people in one country can be more individualistic and collectivistic, on average, than people from another country (Oyserman, 1993;
Cocroft and Ting-Toomey, 1994; Gabrielidis et al.,
1997; Oyserman et al., 2002), cannot be used to challenge his findings because of the conceptual differences inherent in the two levels of analysis (or perhaps methodological artifacts such as acquiescence bias). As we show in our general discussion, different empirical findings across levels (sometimes involving the same cultural values and outcomes) underscore the important theoretical differences between levels.
Researchchallenges
Of the 64 Type I studies at the individual level, only
12 included cultural values other than IND-COL, despite the fact that individuals are affected by a complex set of cultural values (Lytle et al., 1995;
Kirkman and Shapiro, 1997). Indeed, all 12 studies that included cultural values in addition to (or besides) IND-COL found significant effects. Of the five studies that included both IND-COL and other cultural values simultaneously (e.g., Earley, 1986;
Clugston et al., 2000; Mitchell et al., 2000; Kirkman and Shapiro, 2001a; Harpaz et al., 2002), all explained unique variance beyond IND-COL. Such consistent findings suggest that including cultural values other than IND-COL in the other 52 Type I studies would have led to important insights.
Moreover, in the seven Type I studies that examined relationships involving IND-COL and country of origin simultaneously, five of the seven (e.g., Hui et al., 1991; Brett and Okumura, 1998; Gomez et al.,
2000; Kirkman and Shapiro, 2001a; Tinsley and
Brett, 2001) showed that country explained unique variance beyond IND-COL (only Tafarodi et al.
(1999) and Tinsley (2001) showed full mediation effects). This 'hidden ' variance could be explained by other cultural values besides IND-COL (Brett
Journal of International Business Studies
This content downloaded from 130.253.4.14 on Tue, 1 Oct 2013 10:01:32 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
A quarter century of Culture 's Consequences
et
BradleyL Kirkman al
297
et al., 1997). Incorporating different Hofstedeinspired values (e.g., including UA with PD) or competing and complementary conceptualizations, such as Trompenaars ' (1993) concept of universalism-particularism, which refers to the relative salience of rules (universals) or exceptions (particulars), is warranted. Reward allocation decisions could be affected by the extent to which subjects apply rules differently across recipients. Although not specifically discussed above, Kirkman and
Shapiro (2001a) found that Kluckhohn and
Strodtbeck 's (1961) cultural value dimensions of doing-being orientation and free will-determinism explained unique variance beyond IND-COL and PD.
Further, even though researchers see clearer connections between IND-COL (compared with other values) and outcomes such as reward allocation, conflict, negotiation, and individual behavior in groups (Oyserman et al., 2002), the theoretical rationale is too limited. For example, in reward allocation and negotiation studies, the key role of status of the allocator, reward recipient, or negotiators (e.g., Leung and Lind, 1986; Leung, 1997) makes PD important, yet it is rarely assessed.
Indeed, Fischer and Smith 's (2003) meta-analysis showed that PD accounted for more significant cross-cultural differences in reward allocation than
IND-COL (i.e., higher PD cultures allocate rewards using equity, rather than equality, rules). Thus including alternative cultural values is clearly warranted, and future reward allocation studies must at a minimum include PD. Unfortunately, when researchers include cultural values besides
IND-COL, they tend to fall back on Hofstede 's
(1980a) country scores rather than direct, individual level measures (e.g., Bochner and Hesketh,
1994; Mitchell et al., 2000; Steensma et al., 2000a;
Harpaz et al., 2002). We debate the validity of primary vs secondary data in our discussion. Thus, much remains to be done on the direct measurement of PD, UA, MAS-FEM, and Confucian dynamism at the individual level (e.g., see the Type
Type I studies of culture at the group/ organization level of analysis
Type I studies at the group/organization level are fewer in number and more recent than individual level studies. Oetzel (1998) found that individualistic European-American groups had a greater number of conflicts, fewer cooperating tactics, and more competing tactics than collectivistic
II individual level section for examples of the direct measurement of PD), and we recommend incorporating other value dimensions into this level of analysis.
Our review also demonstrated the importance of fine-grained models when conducting Type I studies at the individual level. Again, the reward allocation studies are useful for illustration.
Although some researchers found associations between IND-COL and allocator preferences for
Japanese-American groups. In two studies also discussed in the individual level section, Pillai and
Meindl (1998) found that COL was positively related to charismatic leadership emergence in a field study of 101 work groups; and Eby and
Dobbins (1997) found that team collectivistic orientation was positively related to team cooperation, and team cooperation mediated the relationship between team collectivistic orientation and team performance in a study of 33 student teams.
equal or equitable distributions (Mann et al., 1985;
Leung and Iwawaki, 1988; Hui et al., 1991), other studies found no relationship (McLean-Parks al., et 1999), or found that all subjects preferred equity distributions (Gomez et al., 2000). Including boundary conditions or moderating variables is the key to resolving such conflicting findings. For example, Tower et al. (1997) found that British and
Russian allocators differed in their decision rule only when allocator performance was low rather than high. Similarly, C.C. Chen et al. (1998a, b) found that achievement motivation was significantly negatively related to differential allocation in both HK and the US when task interdependence was high; however, when task interdependence was low, the opposite was true for the US. Similarly, a series of studies showed that the ingroup-outgroup distinction played an important role for collectivists compared with individualists (Hui et al.,
1991; Tower et al., 1997; Gomez et al., 2000).
Such findings show that relationships involving
IND-COL (and, by extension, other cultural values) exist only when certain contextual conditions are present. Leung (1997) drew a similar conclusion when reviewing several reward allocation studies, arguing that interactional goals and situational variables (i.e., the relationship between allocators and recipients and the role assumed by the allocator) interact with culture to affect the allocation rule adopted. Researchers should take these conditions into account to develop better-specified tests of existing theory involving moderators or mediators to explain the contingency relationships of cultural values at individual level.
Journal of International Business Studies
This content downloaded from 130.253.4.14 on Tue, 1 Oct 2013 10:01:32 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
A quarter century of Culture 'sConsequences
et
L
Bradley Kirkmanal
298
Kirkmanand Shapiro (2001b) found that COL was negatively related to the level of team member resistance to the team-related aspects of SMWTs, and that resistance mediated the relationships COL had with team cooperation, empowerment, and productivity. Elron (1997) assigned country scores (Hofstede,
1980a) to top management team (TMT)members to assess cultural heterogeneity in 121 subsidiaries in
34 countries. TMT cultural heterogeneity was positively related to TMTperformance and conflict; however, conflict was negatively related to TMT performance. Cultural heterogeneity on both INDCOL and MAS-FEMwas positively related to TMT performance; and heterogeneity in UA was positively related to conflict. Cox et al. (1991) assessed whether Asians, Blacks, and Hispanics were more collectivistic than Anglos based on the number of cooperative choices individuals made prior to group discussion. Ethnically diverse groups (i.e., those that were high, on average, in COL)behaved more cooperatively than all-Anglo groups (those that were low, on average, in COL), and that these differences tended to increase when situational cues favored cooperation. Of concern was the low reliability of their survey-based COL measure (a mixture of items from Triandis et al., 1986; Hui,
1988). Although Hofstede has offered a different set of dimensions for organizational, rather than national, culture (see Hofstede et al., 1990), two studies adapted his national culture framework for use at the organizational level. Morris et al. (1993) examined 84 manufacturing firms in the US and found a curvilinear relationship between IND-COL and entrepreneurial behavior such that at high levels of either IND or COL entrepreneurial behavior suffers. Morriset al. (1994) replicated the above findings using firms in South Africa, but their findings did not replicate in Portugal (possibly because of relatively high COL in Portugal).
cient (ICC1) or within-and-between-analysis. Typically, there is evidence of high interrateragreement assessed using tests of within-group reliability (rwg) or an ICC2 (e.g., Kirkman et al., 2001). From a pragmatic perspective, as organizations continue to use teams across subsidiariesof multinational firms, we see as very promising research that focuses on how cultural values affect teams and how cultural value diversity plays out at the group/ organization level.
Researchaccomplishments
Even though Hofstede (1980a) conceptualized and
Researchchallenges
Despite the huge increase in the use of teams in both the US and in organizations worldwide (Kirkman and Shapiro, 1997), we found a relative lack of attention to Type I studies at the group/organization level. However, conceptualizing and operationalizing cultural values at the group/organization level might be problematic for several reasons. First, researchers need to grapple with the question of whether culture is functionally isomorphic when moving from one level to the next (Chan, 1998).
Simply put, does the meaning of a cultural value change from the individual to the group/organization level? Clearly, this question is just as relevant for those researcherswho have adopted Hofstede 's
(1980a) country level measures for the individual level. This issue forms the basis for Hofstede 's
(1980a) warning about using his measures at the individual level of analysis; and our review shows that researchers studying the group and organizational levels are as curiously silent on this issue as those working at the individual level. Even if aggregation of individual data can be statistically justified, the importance of conceptually verifying that groups or organizations can indeed hold values that differentiate them from others in a national context cannot be overstated.
Second, cultural values are assumed to develop over time after repeated exposure to multiple facets of culture(s) (Hofstede, 1980a; Brett et al., 1997). It may be unreasonable to assume that because people share membership in a team, there will be more
at the the value dimensions operationalized national level, it is not surprising that researchers have adapted the values for group and organization level studies as national cultures are 'groups, ' and one might reasonably infer that cultural values are equally applicable to smaller groups such as teams and organizations. To justify aggregation, researchers commonly demonstrate more variation between groups than within using an analysis of variance
(ANOVA) procedure, intraclass correlation coeffi-
cultural value similarity within, rather than across, teams. Simply put, can working in teams
(or organizations) alter deeply held, fundamental cultural values (Triandis, 2004)? Researchers must consider when it might be theoretically plausible to develop group or organizational level models involving cultural values before research is conducted, rather than assume similarity among teams. within individuals Similarity within teams or organizations requires accultural processes
journal of International Business Studies
This content downloaded from 130.253.4.14 on Tue, 1 Oct 2013 10:01:32 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
A quarter century of Culture 'sConsequences
L et Bradley Kirkmanal
299
and/or attraction-similarity phenomenon, but these mechanisms are rarely explicitly argued or empirically tested. Further, perhaps the development of cultural values at the group/organization level depends upon team/organization tenure and task characteristics such as interdependence or cohesion.
In addition, whereas diversity studies have grown exponentially (Milliken and Martins, 1996), cultural value diversity has rarelybeen examined in favor of demographics (e.g., age, gender, ethnicity). This exclusion exists despite the finding that deep level diversity (i.e., differences due to values or beliefs) is more important for group functioning than surface level (i.e., demographic) diversity, especially over time (Harrison et al., 1998). Another intriguing question for researchers is what matters more, the mean level of cultural values in a group or group diversity on values? We found no studies that included both facets of cultural values at the group level. Further,as Supplementary Appendix A shows, group level theories were seldom used to build arguments for the impact of group level cultural values. More attention needs to be paid to the underlying dynamics connecting group level cultural values, group processes, and group effectiveness. Theories of collective cognition (e.g., Gibson,
2001), borrowing from social information processing, social cognition, or social identity research, might be effectively used.
Of the five cultural values, IND-COL was included most frequently in group/organization level studies, perhaps because of its close theoretical ties to group behavior. However, links between other cultural values and team processes and performance are equally plausible. For example,
PD could be negatively related to willingness of a team to take autonomous action (Pillai and Meindl,
1998; Kirkman and Shapiro, 2001b). Masculine values (e.g., achievement, assertiveness, not caring for others) may affect cohesiveness (Hofstede,
1998). UA could affect group level phenomena such as risky shift or polarization (i.e., the tendency
Culturaldistance (54)
Most research at this level examined the impact of cultural distance on organizational and country level outcomes. Almost all studies used Kogut and
Singh 's (1988) index, which comprises the differences between a given (subsidiary) country 's score on a cultural value and a (home) country 's score, with differences summed across Hofstede 's cultural values (see Shenkar (2001) and Harzing (2004) for recent critiques)]. Findings demonstrated that as the cultural distance between countries increased, the tendency to choose a joint venture (JV)over an acquisition increased (Kogut and Singh, 1988;
Chang and Rosenzweig, 2001). However, Brouthers and Brouthers (2001) showed that investment risk moderated this relationship such that as risk increases, higher cultural distance is related to preferences for wholly owned entry modes rather than JVs. Also, as cultural distance increased,
Japanese firms were more likely to choose greenfields (Anand and Delios, 1997) or wholly owned subsidiaries (Padmanabhan and Cho, 1996) over shared ownership; the tendency to choose licensing over JVs or wholly owned subsidiaries increased
(Kim and Hwang, 1992); the tendency to choose a greenfield over an acquisition increased (Harzing,
2002); wholly owned subsidiaries were less preferred than either shared-equity ventures (Barkema
for groups to reach more risky or more extreme decisions than individuals). In addition, to assess cultural values at the group level, some studies used individual referents (e.g., 'I value my self-interest over my group interest ' rather than 'My team members value their self-interest over their own interests ') and then aggregated the items to the team level (e.g., Pillai and Meindl, 1998). Such a procedure violates commonly accepted practices for measuring group level constructs, aggregating
and Vermeulen, 1998; Hennart and Larimo, 1998) or technology licensing (Arora and Fosfuri, 2000); the tendency to choose management-service contracts over franchising increased (Erramilli et al., 2002); a greater proportion of incentive-based compensation was used for subsidiary managers of host-country foreign affiliates (Roth and O 'Donnell, 1996); equity JV partners were more likely to acquire an equal or majority (rather than minority) share (Pan, 1996; Erramilli et al., 1997); greater
only those items that have 'group ' or 'team ' referents (Chan, 1998). Thus, scholars should strive for level of analysis alignmentbetween their theoretical foundation, hypotheses, operationalization of constructs, and analyses. If cultural values are assessed at the team level, then all items assessing cultural values should refer to the team, not individuals within the team, and all statistical analyses (e.g., reliability checks, factor analyses, regression) should be conducted using aggregated data.
Type I studies of culture at the country level of analysis
Journal of International Business Studies
This content downloaded from 130.253.4.14 on Tue, 1 Oct 2013 10:01:32 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
0
300
A quarter century of Culture 's Consequences
structural changes in alliance and contracts took place (Kashlak et al., 1998); firms engaged in less
R&D (Richardsand De Carolis, 2003); and a greater number of TMTs departed from US companies acquired by foreign firms (Krugand Nigh, 1998).
In addition, as cultural distance increased, the amount of US foreign direct investment (FDI) decreased (Li and Guisinger, 1992; Loree and
Guisinger, 1995); shareholder wealth in those firms making cross-border acquisitions decreased
(Datta and Puia, 1995); foreign venture longevity decreased (Barkema et al., 1996), especially when
JVs or acquisitions were considered (Barkemaet al.,
1997); the level of embeddedness and integration between host companies and affiliates decreased
(Hakanson and Nobel, 2001); the degree of personal attachment in international cooperative ventures decreased (Luo, 2001a), as did the frequency of expressive ties in organizational networks (Manev and Stevenson, 2001); and the level of CEO role conflict and ambiguity (Gong et al., 2001), international expansion performance (Luo and Peng,
1999), local responsiveness (Luo, 2001b), subsidiary return on assets (Luo and Park, 2001), the payoffs from JV partner buyouts (Reuer, 2001), IJV sales
(Luo, 2002), and the likelihood of success of foreign-owned affiliates in the US (Li and Guisinger,
1991) all decreased. Increasing cultural distance from the US was negatively associated with entrepreneurial traits such as internal locus of control, moderate risk-taking, and high energy level at the country level (Thomas and Mueller, 2000). As the pre-entry international experience of firms increased, so did the likelihood of entering culturally dissimilar countries (Erramilli, 1991). Finally, compared with corporate cultural distance, national cultural distance better predicted stress, negative attitudes towards the merger, and actual cooperation (Weberet al., 1996).
Interestingly, others found that cultural distance did not affect initial or subsequent FDI decisions
(Benito and Gripsrud, 1992); the type of cooperative arrangements of firms across borders (Pan and
Tse, 1996); the choice between acquisitions, greenfields (Brouthers and Brouthers, 2000), alliance performance (Glaister and Buckley, 1999), firm performance (Gomez-Mejia and Palich, 1997), amount of FDI (Habib and Zurawicki, 2002), or abnormal return as a result of US international JV
(Merchant and Schendel,
(IJV) announcement
2000) or after FDIs are made (Kallunki et al.,
2001). On the contrary, larger cultural distances were related to lower JV dissolution rates (Park and
et
BradleyLKirkman
Ungson, 1997), higher IJV(Pothukuchi et al., 2002), and cross-borderacquisition performance (Morosini et al., 1998), decreased preference for greenfields
(Nachum, 2003), and increased FDI into Mexico
(Thomas and Grosse, 2001). Other research examined relationships using country scores on Hofstede 's (1980a) cultural values while controllingfor cultural distance. In these studies, firms preferred
FDI over licensing (Shane, 1992, 1994) and sought majority ownership in foreign subsidiaries (Erramilli, 1996) in high PD countries, and UA was positively linked to majority ownership in foreign subsidiaries (Erramilli, 1996), a tendency to use JVs or greenfields over acquisitions (Kogut and Singh, 1988), and preferences for greenfield start-up ventures over acquisitions (Brouthers and
Brouthers, 2000).
Other country level studies (24)
Beyond cultural distance, other Type I studies examined cultural values at the country level.
Regarding HRM, Newman and Nollen (1996) examined the fit between national culture (using
Hofstede 's country scores) and management practices in 176 European and Asian work units located in 18 countries of a US-based multinational. When managers fitted their practices to a country 's values, the units had higher return on assets and sales and, in some cases, higher bonuses than those with less fit. Findings held for all of the cultural values separately except UA. Using a variety of different worldwide compensation surveys and Hofstede 's country scores, Schuler and Rogovsky (1998) found that IND was positively related to the use of payfor-performance and a focus on individual performance, social benefits such as child care and career breaks, and employee stock ownership plans
PD
(ESOPs). was negatively related to social benefits
ESOPs.UA was positively related to seniorityand and skill-based pay plans and ESOPsand negatively related to a focus on individual performance. MAS was positively related to individual bonuses and commissions, career breaks, and maternity leave, and negatively related to flexible benefits and workplace childcare. From survey data of almost
1,000 companies in 20 countries, Ryan et al. (1999) found that, as UA increased, the number used and extent of verification methods in selection processes decreased (opposite to what was expected).
The number of test types, extent of testing, number of interviews, use of a fixed list of interviews, and number of methods of auditing their selection processes all increased as UA increased. As the level
Journal of International Business Studies
This content downloaded from 130.253.4.14 on Tue, 1 Oct 2013 10:01:32 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
A quarter century of Culture 'sConsequences
L et Bradley Kirkmanal
301
of PD increased, the overall number of interviews used in selection and the extent of peer involvement in hiring increased while the use of peers as interviewers decreased.
Regarding innovation championing strategies, as
UA increased, preferences for champions to work through organizational norms, rules and procedures increased. As PD increased, preferences for champions to focus on gaining the support of those in authority before other actions are taken rather than building a broad base of support increased; and as COLincreased, preferences for champions to seek cross-functional support for innovation increased using survey data from 1,000 employees in 30 countries (Shane et al., 1995). In a study using over 4,000 survey responses in 68 countries,
Shane (1995) found that lower levels of UA were associated with preferences for four innovation championing roles: the organizational maverick, the network facilitator, the transformational leader, and the organizational buffer. The greater legitimacy of these roles suggests that uncertainty acceptance may be linked to more innovative societies. Regarding leadership, PD was negatively related to leader communication, approachability, delegation, and team building; and UA was positively related to leader control and negatively to delegation and approachability using survey data of over 400 managers in a single multinational firm representing 39 different countries (Offerman and
Hellmann, 1997). COL was positively associated with team-oriented leadership, and PD and UA were negatively associated with participative leadership in a sample of middle managers representing 54 countries at both societal and organizational levels (House et al., 1999). The authors note that all of the cultural value items were phrased using a 'should-be ' orientation rather than a reflection of actual practices (p 217).
In studies of societal outcomes, using data from
18 countries common to both Hofstede (1980a) and the Chinese Values Survey (CVS; Chinese Culture
Connection, 1987), Franke et al. (1991) found that
subjective well-being of nations in a sample of undergraduate students in 55 nations. UA was negatively related to the use of professional accounting standards, and positively related to uniformity, conservatism, and secrecy in 29 countries (Salter and Niswander, 1995). MAS was positively related to uniformity and negatively related to conservatism, and IND was negatively related to secrecy. Ronkainen and Guerrero-Cusumano (2001) found that countries with higher
COL, PD, and UA had higher rates of intellectual property rights violations in data from 50 countries
(using Hofstede 's (1980a) country scores).
Regardingalliance formation and entry modes (in studies that did not control for cultural distance), firms in high MAScultures were less likely to pursue technology alliances than were firms in high FEM cultures; and firms in high IND cultures were less likely to pursue equity ties in their alliance formation in a sample of 494 manufacturing firms in
Australia, Indonesia, Mexico, Norway, and Sweden
(Steensma et al., 2000b). Firms from countries with large PD prefer subsidiary and equity JV entry modes whereas firms from countries high in UA prefer contract agreements and export entry modes, based on country scores from Hofstede (1980a), for
10,000 foreign entry activities into China between
1979 and 1998 (Pan and Tse, 2000). Firms from countries higher in UA preferred higher levels of equity ownership in 8,078 IJVsin China between
1979 and 1996 (Pan, 2002).
An interesting set of exchanges regarding relationships between national culture and role conflict, ambiguity, and overload took place in the
Academy of Management Journal. Peterson et al.
(1995) found that high PD and low IND were positively related to high levels of role overload and low levels of role ambiguity. In a challenge to
Peterson et al. 's (1995) findings, Van de Vliert and
Van Yperen (1996) found that average daytime temperature for a country 's capital was positively related to role overload, and that PD scores were not significant once ambient temperature was
Confucian dynamism was positively associated with economic growth between 1965-1980 and
1980-1987. IND was also positively related, but only in the first time period. IND was positively correlated with national wealth in a study of over
13,000 undergraduate students in 31 nations
(Diener and Diener, 1995). PD, UA, and MAS were positively related to the level of corruption in over 40 countries (Husted, 1999). Diener et al.
(1995) found that IND was strongly related to the
entered. In a reply, Peterson and Smith (1997) added 11 new countries and replaced the capital city temperature with the temperature from the cities in which their data were actually collected.
Results show that PD was more strongly associated with role overload than ambient temperature, and that the findings extended also to role ambiguity.
Finally, the remaining country level studies examined various relationships between national culture and outcomes. National culture was asso-
Journal of International Business Studies
This content downloaded from 130.253.4.14 on Tue, 1 Oct 2013 10:01:32 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
A quarter century of Culture 's Consequences
et
L
Bradley Kirkman al
302
Researchaccomplishments
The research above implies that decisions on how to expand internationally are influenced by how different participating countries are from each other on cultural value (country) scores. More generally, the findings reinforce Hofstede 's (1980a) original contention that values are related to the aggregate management practices and beliefs of nations. The important findings regarding the fit
Researchchallenges
The most glaring need in Type I studies at the country level is to explain the many conflicting findings regarding cultural distance and decisions such as entry mode choice. As mentioned at the beginning of this section, Shenkar (2001) provided a scathing theoretical and methodological critique of the Kogut and Singh (1988) measure of cultural distance, and speculated that a number of 'hidden assumptions ' could be at the root of the many conflicting findings with regardto FDIstudies. One of these assumptions is the 'illusion of stability. '
It is possible that, over time, Hofstede 's (1980a) country scores used to create the cultural distance indices have lost predictive validity (we discuss this possibility further in the discussion). If no longer applicable, perhaps an alternative to the country score index is individual perceptions of differences
(Shenkar (2001); see Kim and Hwang (1992) and
Luo et al. (2001) for examples]. Another possibility
[not considered by Shenkar (2001)] is that important moderators have been omitted. For example,
Brouthers and Brouthers (2001) found that investment risk in the target market moderated the relationship between cultural distance and entry mode explaining the conflicting findings of previous studies. We urge researchers to continue to look for theoretically supported moderators of the cultural distance-outcome relationships. Other studies showed that examining 'distance ' on each of the cultural values separately is important. For example, cultural distance on IND-COL and UA were negatively related to CEO role ambiguity
(Shenkar and Zeira, 1992), UA and long-term orientation were negatively related to IJV survival
(Barkema and Vermeulen, 1997), and IND was positively related to US research and development investments abroad (Jones and Teegen, 2001).
Pothukuchi et al. (2002) found the positive relationship between overall cultural distance and IJV performance was explained primarily by cultural distance on MAS. Thus, findings for cultural distance can be explained by including specific
between national culture and management practices (Newman and Nollen, 1996) demonstrated that being culturally sensitive pays (i.e., with higher returns on assets, sales, and higher bonuses). These 'fit ' findings echo recent studies showing positive in Mexico, Poland, affects for empowerment and the US, but negative outcomes in India
(Robert et al., 2000) and managerial reluctance to empower subordinates in high PD cultures (Aycan et al., 2000).
cultural value dimensions independently (Shenkar,
2001). As Shenkar (2001) also noted, with regard to international expansion choices, perhaps UA is more important than the other cultural values thanks to its theoretical link with attitudes towards risk and formalization (e.g., see Barkema et al.
(1997) for empirical tests of this possibility). Given all of these important findings, we take Shenkar 's
(2001) critique a step further (i.e., he recommended simply supplementing the Kogut and Singh (1988)
ciated with whether a strategic case issue was seen as a crisis, as stimulating, as a threat, the future better if resolved, as difficult to resolve, quick action needed to resolve, one correct solution, and as seen as an opportunity based on survey data from 303 MBA students and executives in 16 countries that were grouped into five country clusters (Schneider and DeMeyer, 1991). Specifically, Latin Europeans were most likely to interpret the issue as a crisis and a threat. In a 25-country study, those highest in IND exhibited the lowest levels of information-seeking in individual networks (Zaheer and Zaheer, 1997). IND and PD were positively (and UA negatively) related to a country 's systematic risk in its stock exchange in a 16-country study using Hofstede 's (1980a) country scores
(Riahi-Belkaoui, 1998). In a 23-country study,
Smith et al. (1998) found that PD was negatively related to the frequency of outgroup disagreements; within collectivistic nations, disagreements were more frequently handled through reliance on rules rather than personal experience or training (the opposite was found for individualistic nations); and within low PD nations, ingroup disagreements were handled more frequently through reliance on subordinates whereas outgroup disagreements were more frequently handled through reliance on peers
(compared with reliance on supervisors). Finally,
Spector et al. (2001b), using survey data from over
5,000 managers in 24 countries, found that IND was positively related to both internal locus of control and job satisfaction.
Journal of International Business Studies
This content downloaded from 130.253.4.14 on Tue, 1 Oct 2013 10:01:32 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
A quarter century of Culture 'sConsequences
measure with Confucian dynamism scores), we strongly encourage researchers to avoid furtheruse of the overallculturaldistanceindex.
Type II studies of culture at the individual level of analysis Studies examining the moderating effects of culture at the individual level of analysis currently take three forms (from least to most analytically rigorous): (1) testing whether people from various countries are significantly different on cultural values and then, on the basis of these differences, using country as a moderator;
(2) testing the moderating effects of culture using actual assessments of cultural values; or
(3) after testing the moderating effects of cultural values, determining whether or not country explains additional variance.
Clearly, the last approach is the most valuable, because researchers can show that cultural values account for country variation. Moderator effects imply that theories and practices for managing people need to be altered based on cultural contingencies. We organize these 24 moderating studies into the traditional management/psychology categories of negotiation, leadership, individual behavior relating to group processes, workrelated attitudes, motivation, and organizational justice. et
BradleyLKirkmanal
303
held for those higher rather than lower in HIND; and influence targets higher rather than lower in
VCOL also benefited more when the influence agent held a high status position in the group.
Leadership(2)
Jung and Avolio (1999) examined Asian and US
American students working on a brainstorming task
(Asians were significantly higher in COL than US
Americans). Country moderated the relationship between leadership style and quantity of ideas such that Asian students generated more ideas working with a transformational leader than with a transactional leader (the opposite was true for US students). There were no statistical tests of INDCOL. A second study showed that IND-COL moderated the relationship between perceived uncertainty and the odds of using alliances such that the relationship was stronger when managers were collectivistic rather than individualistic (Dickson and Weaver, 1997).
Negotiation (2)
Gelfand and Realo (1999) conducted dyad negotiation experiments with Caucasian and Asian American undergraduate students in the US (Asians were significantly higher on COL) and undergraduate students in Estonia (who did not significantly differ on COL from US subjects). COL moderated the relationship between accountability and profit from negotiations such that in high accountability negotiations, the more collectivistic the dyad, the higher the level of willingness to concede, coopera-
Individualbehavior relating to group processes (4)
After confirming that managerial trainees in the
PRC were significantly higher on COL than US participants, in a three-way interaction, Earley
(1989) found that COL moderated the relationship between both accountability and shared responsibility and performance such that highly individualistic people performed poorest under conditions of high shared responsibility and low accountability, whereas highly collectivistic people performed better under conditions of high shared responsibility, regardlessof accountability. Country did not explain any unique variance in performance beyond COL, and similar results were obtained when country was substituted for COL. In a followup experiment conducted with PRC,Israeli, and US managers (Earley, 1993), IND-COL moderated the relationship between group condition (i.e., working in an ingroup, outgroup or alone) and individual performance such that the performance of individualists who thought they were working in an
tive behavior, profit from the negotiation, and positive impressions of one 's opponent. In low accountability negotiations, COL was negatively associated with these outcomes. Ng and Van Dyne
(2001) found that HIND and HCOL moderated the between minority influence and relationship improvement in decision quality such that, in groups with minority influence, individuals higher rather than lower in HCOL were less likely to improve their decision quality. These results also
ingroup or an outgroup was lower than the performance of individualists working alone; the performance of collectivists was lower in an individual or outgroup context than in an ingroup context; and participant ratings of self or group efficacy and their anticipated performance outcomes mediated the effects of IND-COL on performance. Y. Chen et al. (1998b) examined the effects of individual vs collective primacy (referring to whether people give more weight to their personal
Journal of International Business Studies
This content downloaded from 130.253.4.14 on Tue, 1 Oct 2013 10:01:32 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
304
A quarter century of Culture 's Consequences
et
BradleyLKirkman al
interests than to their ingroup 's interests when forced to choose between the two) and found that undergraduates from the PRC (who had more collective primacy orientation than those from the US) exhibited more ingroup favoritism when they performed well individually while their group performed poorly. Results were replicated substituting collective primacy for country and, in some cases, country no longer explained variance after collective primacy was entered. Wagner (1995) found that IND-COL moderated the relationships between both size and identifiability and cooperation such that size and identifiability have stronger effects on the cooperation of individualists than collectivists in a sample of US undergraduates.
of job alternatives and job satisfaction and the behavioral responses of exit, voice, loyalty, and neglect. Specifically, horizontal individualists were more sensitive to quality of job alternatives when considering whether or not to exit; high levels of
VCOL enhanced the relationship between quality of alternatives and exit; and quality of alternatives and voice were positively related at high, but not low, levels of HIND. For employees high in VCOL, job satisfaction was positively related to loyalty, but the relationship was stronger when quality of alternatives was high (opposite for employees low in VCOL).
northern Italians, Martella and Maass (2000) found that region moderated the relationship between being unemployed and having lower life satisfaction, self-esteem, and happiness such that the relationships were stronger for northern rather than southern Italians (IND-COL, however, was never entered as a moderator). Finally, in a study discussed in the Type I individual level section,
Thomas and Au (2002) found that HIND and VCOL moderated the relationships between both quality
three countries, individualists responded more training than favorably to individual-focused group-focused training, and collectivists showed the opposite pattern. Importantly, country of origin had no effect beyond COL. In a sample of MBA students in Canada in an experiment that manipulated empowerment, Eylon and Au (1999) found that PD (scores assigned based on language and country of origin) moderated the relationship between empowerment and performance such that
Motivation (5)
After finding that US subjects were significantly lower on both COL and PD than Israelis, Erez and
Work-related
attitudes (5)
In a sample of nearly 2,000 managers from 15
Earley (1987) found that country moderated the relationship between type of goal (assigned, repreEuropean and Canadian affiliates of a US multisentative, or participative) and performance such national, Palich et al. (1995) did not find moderating effects for IND-COL, PD, UA, or MAS-FEM that the relationships between both representative and participative goal-setting and performance
(using Hofstede 's country scores) on the relationwere stronger in Israel than the US. Identical between typical organizational commitment ships findings were obtained when PD (but not COL) predictors (i.e., job scope, participative managewas substituted for country. In a sample of ment, extrinsic rewards,and role clarity) and actual commitment. Similarly, in a study of European Mexican, Taiwanese and US managers working in
Mexico and Taiwan, Dorfman and Howell (1988)
Commission employees representing 12 countries found that cultural socialization (i.e., strong beliefs and using country scores, no moderating effects in the key cultural values of a society) moderated were found for the relationship between various the relationship between directive leadership and of commitment (i.e., affective, normacomponents both subordinate performance and satisfaction and continuance) and intention to quit tive, such that for individuals with high, rather than
(Vandenberghe et al., 2001). Schaubroeck et al. low, cultural socialization, directive leadership had
(2000), after demonstrating that US bank tellers were significantly higher on IND and lower on COL stronger relationships to outcomes; but the effects of contingent reward leadership behaviors than HK tellers, found that perceiving higher remained invariant regardless of the levels of control mitigated the relationship between job cultural socialization. Earley (1994) found that demands and both psychological health symptoms and turnover intentions among US bank tellers country moderated the effects of type of training
(individual- vs group-focused) on self-efficacy and reporting high job self-efficacy. In the HKsample, it was collective (not self-) efficacy that showed this performance such that self-focused training had a of effects. Similar results emerged when stronger impact on self-efficacy and performance pattern for US subjects whereas group-focused training had
IND-COL for country. After finding substituting that southern Italians were more collectivistic than stronger effects for HK and PRCsubjects. Within all
journal of International Business Studies
This content downloaded from 130.253.4.14 on Tue, 1 Oct 2013 10:01:32 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
A quarter century of Culture 'sConsequences
for participants in the high PD group the disempowerment condition was associated with higher job performance than was the empowered or control condition; conversely, participants low in PD performed similarly across empowerment conditions. PD group (PD was never directly assessed) did not moderate the empowerment-job satisfaction relationship. Finally, Lam et al. (2002a) found that
COL moderated the relationship between participative decision-making and individual performance, such that they were positively related for those high in IND (US employees were significantly higher on IND and lower on COL than HK employees). Importantly, country did not account for significant variance after IND had been entered.
Organizationaljustice (6)
In an experiment, Brockner et al. (2000) showed that when procedural fairness was high, outcome favorability had less of an impact on reactions to a decision in Taiwan than in Canada. Results were generally replicated substituting IND-COL (i.e., independent vs interdependent self-construal) for country, and country failed to account for significant variance beyond IND-COL. Brockner et al.
(2001) found that country moderated the relationship between voice and commitment such that participants were more likely to respond unfavorably (i.e., with lower levels of commitment) to low levels of voice when they were in lower (e.g., the US and Germany), rather than higher (e.g., the PRC and Mexico) PD countries. Similarto Brockneret al.
(2000), results were replicated when PD was substituted for country, and country had no effects beyond PD. Also, similar results were obtained in the PRC for job satisfaction, intent to remain, and job performance. In contrast, Lind et al. (1997) found that the relationship between voice and perceptions of procedural justice was equally strong among students in Germany, HK,Japan, and the US
(however, country was used as the moderator, not
PD). Au et al. (2001), after confirming that Canadian undergraduates were significantly higher on
IND than those from the PRC, found that country moderated the relationship between voice and taking responsibility such that when voice was offered by a service provider, Canadians were less likely to attribute the responsibility to themselves than were the Chinese (however, there were no tests of IND-COL). In a study of HK bank tellers,
Lee et al. (2000b) found that PD moderated the relationships between both distributive and procedural justice and both trust in supervisor and
et
L
BradleyKirkmanal
305
psychological contract fulfillment such that the relationship between procedural justice and trust in supervisor was stronger for those lower, rather than higher, in PD; and the relationship between distributive justice and psychological contract fulfillment was stronger for those lower, rather than higher, in PD. Finally, Lam et al. (2002b) found that PD (but not IND) moderated the relationship between both distributive and procedural justice and work outcomes such that the relationships between the two types of justice and job satisfaction, performance, and absenteeism were more strongly positive for those lower, rather than higher in PD. HK bank tellers were significantly higher in
PD than US tellers, but there were no moderating effects for country.
Researchaccomplishments
Like Type I research at the individual level,
IND-COL was the most frequently examined value in Type II studies. Whether examining within-group or between-groupbehavior, collectivists were more likely to cooperate than were individualists, especially when accountability was high. Reinforcing the notion that collectivists place a higher value on ingroup, rather than outgroup, well-being, several studies demonstrated that the performance of collectivists was affected more by ingroup-outgroup status than individualists ' performance.
These findings echo those from the Type I section.
There were, however, several key differences. First, the number of moderating studies is increasing rapidly (over half were published between 1999 and
2002), whereas the publishing rate for main effect studies remained stable. Second, of the six studies that included country and IND-COL in analyses simultaneously, all found that country no longer had a significant effect after IND-COLwas entered.
A seventh study (Brockner et al., 2001) found the same results with PD. Thus, when culture is a moderator, it is possible to specify the influence of a particularly potent dimension, and this dimension is likely to single-handedly account for differences across countries. This was not the case in main effects research, where a single cultural value very rarely explained all of the variation across countries.
Research challenges
Beyond IND-COL, PD was the only value examined as a moderator. We urge more Type II research on variation on PD
PD. Perhaps within-culture the expected relationship between explains why
journal of International Business Studies
This content downloaded from 130.253.4.14 on Tue, 1 Oct 2013 10:01:32 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
306
A quarter century of Culture 's Consequences
participative goal-setting and both satisfaction and performance has not been consistently supported in the US. We urge more studies on employee participation (e.g., Lam et al., 2002a), utilizing all of the cultural value dimensions as possible moderators. It will be important for future research to examine the moderating effects of UA, for example, on receptivity to virtual work, and the moderating effect of Confucian dynamism, for example, on employee responses to downsizing and other economically focused initiatives. Finally, more research is needed to determine whether the lack of moderating effects for commitment (e.g.,
Palich et al., 1995) and turnover intentions (Vandenberghe et al., 2001) is due to the overall difficulty of detecting moderators (McClelland and Judd, 1993), using country scores instead of direct measures, or a high level of cultural invariance on these outcomes.
et
BradleyL Kirkman al
that IND-COLmoderated the relationship between participative decision-making and group performance such that they were positively related for groups high, but not low, in COL and high participation efficacy. Importantly, country no longer accounted for significant effects beyond
IND-COL.
By experimentally manipulating organizational cultures that were either individualistic or collectivistic and then assessing the cultural values of their
US MBA subjects, Chatman and Barsade (1995) showed that individualistic-collectivistic organizational culture moderated the relationship between personal cooperation and cooperative behavior such that cooperative subjects in collectivistic cultures were more cooperative than individualistic subjects in the individualistic culture, individualistic subjects in a collectivistic culture, and collectivistic subjects in an individualistic culture.
Individualists displayed similar cooperation regardless of organizational culture. Again using US
MBAs,Chatman et al. (1998) found that individualistic-collectivistic organizational culture moderated the relationships between relational demography and social interaction, conflict, productivity, and creativity such that in collectivistic cultures demographic heterogeneity was positively related to communication; more conflict was found in individualistic than in collectivistic cultures, but collectivists viewed conflict as more beneficial; and as demographic similarity decreased, subjects in collectivistic cultures perceived themselves as more creative than those in individualistic cultures; similar people were more productive in individualistic than in collectivistic cultures, whereas dissimilar people were equally productive across the two cultures.
Type II studies of culture at the group/ organization level of analysis
A greater variety of cultural values was used in the group/organization level Type II studies relative to
Type I. Regarding group efficacy, in a laboratory study of US and HK students and a field study of nursing teams in the US and Indonesia, Gibson
(1999) found that COL moderated the relationship between group efficacy and group performance such that they were positively related when COL was high, but not low. Based on a simulation study of managers from England, France, Thailand and the US, Earley (1999) found that PD moderated the relationship between team members ' personal estimates of group efficacy and collective judgments of group efficacy such that in high PD cultures, collective judgments of group efficacy were more strongly tied to higher, rather than lower, status group members ' personal judgments.
In low PD cultures, group members contributed comparably to group efficacy judgments. After finding that kibbutzim managers were higher in
COL than urban managers in Israel, Erez and
Researchaccoplishments
Experimental studies clearly show that COL contributes to collective efficacy, group performance, and cooperative behavior. Further, collectivistic organizational culture is related to conflict percep-
Somech (1996) found that the kibbutzim vs urban split moderated the relationship between both type of goal (individual vs group) and incentive (individual vs group) and group performance such that performance loss was less likely in kibbutzim than in urban groups. The highest level of performance was in kibbutzim groups with a group goal and group incentive (however, there were no direct tests of IND-COL). In a study discussed in the Type II individual level section, Lam et al. (2002a) found
tions. Studies in this category demonstrated that group behavior is complex and often contextdependent (Earley and Gibson, 2002). Therefore, researchers need to continue to explore cultural moderators of group level relationships, with culture as an important contextual contingency condition. Further, in practice, as organizations become more global, they often do so using collaborative, team-based approaches (Kirkman and Shapiro, 1997). Practitioners struggle with the
journal of International Business Studies
This content downloaded from 130.253.4.14 on Tue, 1 Oct 2013 10:01:32 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
A quarter century of Culture 'sConsequences
level of global integration and local responsiveness.
The study of moderators at the group level can help inform these decisions. We would also argue that researchers examining group level Type I effects should emulate the research examining Type II effects reviewed in this section in terms of the wider variety of cultural values investigated (i.e., beyond the typical IND-COL or PD) and experimental methods.
Researchchallenges
As models are developed using moderators as key contingency factors, both theory development and advice to practitioners becomes increasingly narrow, applying to only a small population of employees (e.g., if a team with X values is managed using Y technique under Z conditions, then we can expect success). Therefore, practical considerations and parsimony of models should be considered alongside the explanatory richness of moderator models at the group level. Similarto our criticism in the Type I group level section, we note that, with the exception of Earley (1999) and Gibson (1999), researchers often aggregated items with individual referents to the group level, violating a key requirement for assessing group level phenomena.
We strongly encourage greater attention to such important methodological details to strengthen the robustness of research in this category.
L et Bradley Kirkmanal
307
individualistic countries (Diener et al., 2000).
Spector et al. (2002) found that IND-COL did not moderate the relationship between locus of control and well-being (using the same data from Spector et al., 2001b). Finally, in a study of 295 IJVsin the
PRC, cultural distance moderated the relationship between control and performance such that when cultural distance from the host country was higher, rather than lower, there was a weaker positive relationship between control and performance for foreign, but not Chinese, parents (Luo et al., 2001).
Researchaccomplishments
Type II country level studies show that Hofstede 's cultural values have important effects on micro and macro level relationships across countries.
We believe that this is true because country level phenomena are far removed (i.e., distal) antecedents for the relationships being examined. Yet some of the trends uncovered may be important factors to consider, particularlyas economists begin to incorporate large, survey-based approaches to studying economic implications of macro-human resource phenomena at the country level (e.g.,
Gibbs and Levenson, 2002).
Researchchallenges
What is most glaring about the Type II country level studies is how few there are (five), compared with Type I studies at this level (78). Perhaps this is due, in part, to a trend initiated by Kogut and
Singh (1988) to conceptualize country level cultural distance as a main effect, which may have led subsequent researchers to exclusively investigate such effects (in 54 studies), rather than moderating effects. As demonstrated by Luo et al. (2001) above, however, cultural distance did have interesting effects as a moderator. Thus, main effects researchers should at least control for the possibility.
Type II studies of culture at the country level of analysis
Few studies examined country level moderating effects, which seems curious for a framework that was conceived to explain country differences. In two studies discussed in the Type I country level section, Diener and Diener (1995) found that INDCOL moderated the relationship between both friendship satisfaction and life satisfaction, and between satisfaction with self and life satisfaction, such that the relationships were stronger in individualistic, rather than collectivistic, nations; and Steensma et al. (2000b) found that the relation-
What has been learned, and where do we go
ship between perceived technological uncertainty and the use of technology alliances is stronger for firms in higher, rather than lower, UA countries and for firms in lower, rather than higher, MAS countries. In a 35-study meta-analysis, Robie et al.
(1998) found that PD moderated the relationship between job level and job satisfaction such that it is weaker in lower PD countries. IND-COL moderated the relationship between marriage and life satisfaction such that it is stronger in collectivistic than
from here?
Perhaps the most pertinent question we should ask after conducting a comprehensive review is: should
Hofstede 's (1980a) cultural values framework continue to be used for cross-cultural research in the
21st century? Can a framework based on data collected in the 1960s and early 1970s continue to add value to the international management/ psychology fields? In general, we agree with Smith and Bond 's (1999, 56) conclusion that large-scale
Journal of International Business Studies
This content downloaded from 130.253.4.14 on Tue, 1 Oct 2013 10:01:32 AM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
0 lA
?
308
quarter century of Culture 'sConsequences
studies published since Hofstede 's (1980a) work
(including Chinese Culture Connection, 1987;
Schwartz, 1992, 1994; Trompenaars, 1993; Smith et al., 1996 as examples) 'have sustained and amplified [Hofstede 's] conclusions rather than contradicted them. ' Further, our review shows that researchers have used Hofstede 's framework successfully to select countries that are culturally different in order to increase variance, and that most country differences predicted by Hofstede were supported. Thus, overall, Hofstede 's values are clearly relevant for additional cross-cultural research. However, we temper our enthusiasm with several warnings for researchers. Our concluding section identifies theoretical and methodological implications and gaps in research that represent potential opportunities for future researchers.
L et Bradley Kirkmanal
Similarities and differences in relationships across levels Although the small number of group/organization level studies constrained our detection of relationships across levels, we still found evidence for both cross-level similarities and differences. Regarding similarities, COL was associated with more cooperation and positive attitudes toward teams at the individual (Bennett, 1999; Eby et al., 2000; Kirkman and Shapiro, 2000, 2001a; Steensma et al., 2000a;
Wade-Benzoni et al., 2002), group/organization
(Chatman and Barsade, 1995; Eby and Dobbins,
1997; Kirkman and Shapiro, 2001b), and country levels (Schuler and Rogovsky, 1998; Steensma et al., 2000b). Similarly, IND was associated with increased conflict at the individual (Gabrielidis et al., 1997) and group/organization levels (Cox et al., 1991; Chatman et al., 1998; Oeztzel, 1998, and handling such conflict using personal experience or training rather than formal rules at the country level (Smith et al., 1998). This may help to explain why team efforts often fail in highly individualistic countries such as the US (Kirkman
dissolution of IJVs at the country level (Barkema and Vermeulen, 1997). These similar findings across levels aside, we note that the substantiation of cross-level similarities is actually quite rarein the
Hofstede-inspired research, not necessarily because they do not exist but because researchers seldom use a cross-level approach in a single study (exceptions included Eby and Dobbins, 1997; Pillai and
Meindl, 1998; Lam et al., 2002a). As discussed below, we view such studies as promising, because they address multiple rival explanations for consistency across levels.
There was also evidence for differences across levels, even when considering the same values and outcomes. For example, at the individual level,
COL was positively related to job satisfaction
(Kirkman and Shapiro, 2001a), but at the country level the opposite was true (Spectoret al., 2001b). At the group/organization level, IND-COL had a curvilinear relationship with entrepreneurship
(Morris et al., 1993), but at the country level, increasing cultural distance from the US in terms of
IND-COL (i.e., higher COL) was negatively related to entrepreneurial traits (Thomas and Mueller,
2000). At the individual level, cultural distance on
COL was negatively related to group outcomes
(receptiveness) (Thomas, 1999), but at the group/ organization level, heterogeneity on COL (a concept similar to cultural distance) was positively related to group outcomes (i.e., performance)
(Elron, 1997). Differences were also found for associations among cultural values. Hofstede
(1980a) reported a correlation of -0.67 (P
Citations: Hofstede, 2001). Trompenaars (1993, iii), who has a competing framework, credits Hofstede 'for opening management 's eyes to the inform future research, rather than provide an in-depth discussion of Hofstede 's original study, a critique (e.g., Schwartz, 1994; Smith and Bond, 1999; McSweeney, 2002; Smith, 2002), or a replication This content downloaded from 130.253.4.14 on Tue, 1 Oct 2013 10:01:32 AM A quarter century of Culture 'sConsequences (e.g., Punnett and Withane (1990); Shackleton and All, 1990; Merritt, 2000; Spector et al., 2001a). example, most researchers focused exclusively on individualism-collectivism (IND-COL) at the individual level of analysis (e.g., Triandis, 1995; Earley and Gibson, 1998; Oyserman et al., 2002). to the individual level, Hofstede 'ignores everything.., but the culture level comparisons ' (Smith, 2002, 123), thus missing an opportunity to draw Hofstede (1980a, 25) defined culture as 'the collective programming of the mind which distinguishes the members of one human group from another '. ingroup to look after them, and in exchange for that they feel they owe absolute loyalty to it '(Hofstede, 1980b, 45) and organizations is distributed unequally ' (1980b, 45) the quality of life, or people ' (1980b, 46) and FEMdefined as the opposite of MAS.Michael Harris Bond (Chinese Culture Connection, 1987) and later Hofstede and Bond (1988) developed a fifth
You May Also Find These Documents Helpful
-
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms…
- 10027 Words
- 41 Pages
Powerful Essays -
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .…
- 4346 Words
- 18 Pages
Powerful Essays -
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms…
- 13057 Words
- 53 Pages
Powerful Essays -
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms…
- 4557 Words
- 19 Pages
Powerful Essays -
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .…
- 1290 Words
- 6 Pages
Powerful Essays -
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms…
- 2635 Words
- 9 Pages
Powerful Essays -
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms…
- 8955 Words
- 36 Pages
Good Essays -
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of…
- 6149 Words
- 25 Pages
Powerful Essays -
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of…
- 3436 Words
- 53 Pages
Powerful Essays -
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms…
- 12815 Words
- 37 Pages
Powerful Essays -
Taylor & Francis, Ltd. is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to The Clearing…
- 2955 Words
- 12 Pages
Powerful Essays -
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms…
- 7132 Words
- 36 Pages
Powerful Essays -
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms…
- 6917 Words
- 28 Pages
Powerful Essays -
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms…
- 12396 Words
- 50 Pages
Powerful Essays -
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .…
- 3365 Words
- 14 Pages
Powerful Essays