Questionnaires or social surveys are a method used to collect standardised data from large numbers of people -i.e. the same information is collected in the same way. They are used to collect data in a statistical form.
In Data Collection in Context (1981), Ackroyd and Hughes identify three types of survey:
1. Factual surveys: used to collect descriptive information, i.e. the government census
2. Attitude surveys - i.e. an opinion poll - rather than attempting to gather descriptive information, an attitude survey will attempt to collect and measure people's attitudes and opinions, i.e. 4 out of 5 people believe...
3. Explanatory survey - goes beyond the collection of data and aims to test theories and hypotheses and / or to produce new theory.
Researchers usually use questionnaires or surveys in order that they can make generalisations, therefore, the surveys are usually based on carefully selected samples.
Questionnaires consist of the same set of questions that are asked in the same order and in the same way in order that the same information can be gathered.
Questionnaires can be:
1. Filled in by the participant
2. Asked in a structured and formal way by an interviewer
1. Interviewer bias must be considered when done in this way, however, an advantage of this method over a participant filling in a questionnaire is that the interviewer may assist if there are any ambiguous questions or if the participant is confused in any way
3. Postal questionnaire can be used, whereby a questionnaire is posted to the sample group and returned to the researcher by a specified time and date
4. Administration of a questionnaire to a group is an option - i.e. at centre, school or group. The researcher needs to consider if the group will affect each other's responses and the concentration levels etc when undertaking this approach
5. Telephone questionnaire
6. Email questionnaire
7. Developing a Questionnaire
Developing a Questionnaire
The process of developing a questionnaire involves the following four steps:
1. Choosing the questions by operationalising concepts, which involves translating abstract ideas into concrete questions that will be measureable (i.e......class, power, family, religion....add some sort of example)
2. Operationalising concepts involves a set of choices regarding the following:
1. units of analysis
1. units that can be analysed:
1. individuals (i.e. students, voters, workers)
2. groups (families, gangs)
3. organisations (churches, army, corporations)
4. social artefacts (buildings, cars, pottery, etc)
2. points of focus
3. treatment of the dimension of time
4. nature of measurement
3. Establish an operational definition which involves breaking the concept down into various components or dimensions in order to specify what is to be measured
4. Once the concept has been operationally defined in terms of a number of components, the second step involves the selection of indicatorsfor each component.'
5. '...indicators of each dimension are put into the form of a series of questions that will provide quantifiable data for measuring each dimension.'
8. Questionnaire Questions
Questions in the questionnaire can then be:
1. Open ended (more difficult to extract quantifiable data)
1. This form of question requires the researcher to code the answers. Coding identifies a number of categories in which people have responded, more detail of this process is covered in the qualitative research unit
2. Closed
3. Fixed-choice
4. Likert scale - where participants are given a range of options, i.e. agree, strongly agree...for more information about the Likert scale and other scales of measurement, visit http://www.socialresearchmethods.net/kb/scallik.php
5. the difficulty or negative of all of the close and fixed are that participants may be forced into an answer or may not be able to qualify or explain what they mean by what they have answered
The following links provide further information about social surveys and questionnaires:
http://www.socialresearchmethods.net/kb/survey.php
Refer back to the 'Evaluation Toolkit for the Voluntary and Community Arts in Northern Ireland' and read the section on developing a questionnaire, pages 39 - 42 http://www.artscouncil-ni.org/departs/all/report/VoluntaryCommunityArtsEvalToolkit.pdf 9. The advantages and disadvantages of questionnaires
The advantages of questionnaires
1. Practical
2. Large amounts of information can be collected from a large number of people in a short period of time and in a relatively cost effective way
3. Can be carried out by the researcher or by any number of people with limited affect to its validity and reliability
4. The results of the questionnaires can usually be quickly and easily quantified by either a researcher or through the use of a software package
5. Can be analysed more 'scientifically' and objectively than other forms of research
6. When data has been quantified, it can be used to compare and contrast other research and may be used to measure change
7. Positivists believe that quantitative data can be used to create new theories and / or test existing hypotheses
The disadvantages of questionnaires
1. Is argued to be inadequate to understand some forms of information - i.e. changes of emotions, behaviour, feelings etc.
2. Phenomenologists state that quantitative research is simply an artificial creation by the researcher, as it is asking only a limited amount of information without explanation
3. Lacks validity
4. There is no way to tell how truthful a respondent is being
5. There is no way of telling how much thought a respondent has put in
6. The respondent may be forgetful or not thinking within the full context of the situation
7. People may read differently into each question and therefore reply based on their own interpretation of the question - i.e. what is 'good' to someone may be 'poor' to someone else, therefore there is a level of subjectivity that is not acknowledged
8. There is a level of researcher imposition, meaning that when developing the questionnaire, the researcher is making their own decisions and assumptions as to what is and is not important...therefore they may be missing something that is of importance
The process of coding in the case of open ended questions opens a great possibility of subjectivity by the researcher
Likert Scaling
Like Thurstone or Guttman Scaling, Likert Scaling is a unidimensional scaling method. Here, I'll explain the basic steps in developing a Likert or "Summative" scale.
Defining the Focus. As in all scaling methods, the first step is to define what it is you are trying to measure. Because this is a unidimensional scaling method, it is assumed that the concept you want to measure is one-dimensional in nature. You might operationalize the definition as an instruction to the people who are going to create or generate the initial set of candidate items for your scale.
Generating the Items. next, you have to create the set of potential scale items. These should be items that can be rated on a 1-to-5 or 1-to-7 Disagree-Agree response scale. Sometimes you can create the items by yourself based on your intimate understanding of the subject matter. But, more often than not, it's helpful to engage a number of people in the item creation step. For instance, you might use some form of brainstorming to create the items. It's desirable to have as large a set of potential items as possible at this stage, about 80-100 would be best.
Rating the Items. The next step is to have a group of judges rate the items. Usually you would use a 1-to-5 rating scale where:
1. = strongly unfavorable to the concept
2. = somewhat unfavorable to the concept
3. = undecided
4. = somewhat favorable to the concept
5. = strongly favorable to the concept
Notice that, as in other scaling methods, the judges are not telling you what they believe -- they are judging how favorable each item is with respect to the construct of interest.
Selecting the Items. The next step is to compute the intercorrelations between all pairs of items, based on the ratings of the judges. In making judgements about which items to retain for the final scale there are several analyses you can do:
Throw out any items that have a low correlation with the total (summed) score across all items
In most statistics packages it is relatively easy to compute this type of Item-Total correlation. First, you create a new variable which is the sum of all of the individual items for each respondent. Then, you include this variable in the correlation matrix computation (if you include it as the last variable in the list, the resulting Item-Total correlations will all be the last line of the correlation matrix and will be easy to spot). How low should the correlation be for you to throw out the item? There is no fixed rule here -- you might eliminate all items with a correlation with the total score less that .6, for example.
For each item, get the average rating for the top quarter of judges and the bottom quarter. Then, do a t-test of the differences between the mean value for the item for the top and bottom quarter judges.
Higher t-values mean that there is a greater difference between the highest and lowest judges. In more practical terms, items with higher t-values are better discriminators, so you want to keep these items. In the end, you will have to use your judgement about which items are most sensibly retained. You want a relatively small number of items on your final scale (e.g., 10-15) and you want them to have high Item-Total correlations and high discrimination (e.g., high t-values).
Administering the Scale. You're now ready to use your Likert scale. Each respondent is asked to rate each item on some response scale. For instance, they could rate each item on a 1-to-5 response scale where:
1. = strongly disagree
2. = disagree
3. = undecided
4. = agree
5. = strongly agree
There are a variety possible response scales (1-to-7, 1-to-9, 0-to-4). All of these odd-numbered scales have a middle value is often labeled Neutral or Undecided. It is also possible to use a forced-choice response scale with an even number of responses and no middle neutral or undecided choice. In this situation, the respondent is forced to decide whether they lean more towards the agree or disagree end of the scale for each item.
The final score for the respondent on the scale is the sum of their ratings for all of the items (this is why this is sometimes called a "summated" scale). On some scales, you will have items that are reversed in meaning from the overall direction of the scale. These are called reversal items. You will need to reverse the response value for each of these items before summing for the total. That is, if the respondent gave a 1, you make it a 5; if they gave a 2 you make it a 4; 3 = 3; 4 = 2; and, 5 = 1.
Example: The Employment Self Esteem Scale
Here's an example of a ten-item Likert Scale that attempts to estimate the level of self esteem a person has on the job. Notice that this instrument has no center or neutral point -- the respondent has to declare whether he/she is in agreement or disagreement with the item.
INSTRUCTIONS: Please rate how strongly you agree or disagree with each of the following statements by placing a check mark in the appropriate box.
Strongly Disagree
Somewhat Disagree
Somewhat Agree
Strongly Agree
1. I feel good about my work on the job.
Strongly Disagree
Somewhat Disagree
Somewhat Agree
Strongly Agree
2. On the whole, I get along well with others at work.
Strongly Disagree
Somewhat Disagree
Somewhat Agree
Strongly Agree
3. I am proud of my ability to cope with difficulties at work.
Strongly Disagree
Somewhat Disagree
Somewhat Agree
Strongly Agree
4. When I feel uncomfortable at work, I know how to handle it.
Strongly Disagree
Somewhat Disagree
Somewhat Agree
Strongly Agree
5. I can tell that other people at work are glad to have me there.
Strongly Disagree
Somewhat Disagree
Somewhat Agree
Strongly Agree
6. I know I'll be able to cope with work for as long as I want.
Strongly Disagree
Somewhat Disagree
Somewhat Agree
Strongly Agree
7. I am proud of my relationship with my supervisor at work.
Strongly Disagree
Somewhat Disagree
Somewhat Agree
Strongly Agree
8. I am confident that I can handle my job without constant assistance.
Strongly Disagree
Somewhat Disagree
Somewhat Agree
Strongly Agree
9. I feel like I make a useful contribution at work.
Strongly Disagree
Somewhat Disagree
Somewhat Agree
Strongly Agree
10. I can tell that my coworkers respect me.
Thurstone Scaling
Thurstone was one of the first and most productive scaling theorists. He actually invented three different methods for developing a unidimensional scale: the method of equal-appearing intervals; the method of successive intervals; and, the method of paired comparisons. The three methods differed in how the scale values for items were constructed, but in all three cases, the resulting scale was rated the same way by respondents. To illustrate Thurstone's approach, I'll show you the easiest method of the three to implement, the method of equal-appearing intervals.
The Method of Equal-Appearing Intervals
Developing the Focus. The Method of Equal-Appearing Intervals starts like almost every other scaling method -- with a large set of statements. Oops! I did it again! You can't start with the set of statements -- you have to first define the focus for the scale you're trying to develop. Let this be a warning to all of you: methodologists like me often start our descriptions with the first objective methodological step (in this case, developing a set of statements) and forget to mention critical foundational issues like the development of the focus for a project. So, let's try this again...
The Method of Equal-Appearing Intervals starts like almost every other scaling method -- with the development of the focus for the scaling project. Because this is a unidimensional scaling method, we assume that the concept you are trying to scale is reasonably thought of as one-dimensional. The description of this concept should be as clear as possible so that the person(s) who are going to create the statements have a clear idea of what you are trying to measure. I like to state the focus for a scaling project in the form of a command -- the command you will give to the people who will create the statements. For instance, you might start with the focus command:
Generate statements that describe specific attitudes that people might have towards persons with AIDS.
You want to be sure that everyone who is generating statements has some idea of what you are after in this focus command. You especially want to be sure that technical language and acronyms are spelled out and understood (e.g., what is AIDS?).
Generating Potential Scale Items. Now, you're ready to create statements. You want a large set of candidate statements (e.g., 80 -- 100) because you are going to select your final scale items from this pool. You also want to be sure that all of the statements are worded similarly -- that they don't differ in grammar or structure. For instance, you might want them each to be worded as a statement which you cold agree or disagree with. You don't want some of them to be statements while others are questions.
For our example focus on developing an AIDS attitude scale, we might generate statements like the following (these statements came from a class exercise I did in my Spring 1997 undergrad class): people get AIDS by engaging in immoral behavior you can get AIDS from toilet seats
AIDS is the wrath of God anybody with AIDS is either gay or a junkie
AIDS is an epidemic that affects us all people with AIDS are bad people with AIDS are real people
AIDS is a cure, not a disease you can get AIDS from heterosexual sex people with AIDS are like my parents you can get AIDS from public toilets women don’t get AIDS
I treat everyone the same, regardless of whether or not they have AIDS
AIDS costs the public too much
AIDS is something the other guy gets living with AIDS is impossible children cannot catch AIDS
AIDS is a death sentence because AIDS is preventable, we should focus our resources on prevention instead of curing
People who contract AIDS deserve it
AIDS doesn't have a preference, anyone can get it.
AIDS is the worst thing that could happen to you.
AIDS is good because it will help control the population.
If you have AIDS, you can still live a normal life.
People with AIDS do not need or deserve our help
By the time I would get sick from AIDS, there will be a cure
AIDS will never happen to me you can't get AIDS from oral sex
AIDS is spread the same way colds are
AIDS does not discriminate
You can get AIDS from kissing
AIDS is spread through the air
Condoms will always prevent the spread of AIDS
People with AIDS deserve what they got
If you get AIDS you will die within a year
Bad people get AIDS and since I am a good person I will never get AIDS
I don't care if I get AIDS because researchers will soon find a cure for it.
AIDS distracts from other diseases that deserve our attention more bringing AIDS into my family would be the worst thing I could do very few people have AIDS, so it's unlikely that I'll ever come into contact with a sufferer if my brother caught AIDS I'd never talk to him again
People with AIDS deserve our understanding, but not necessarily special treatment
AIDS is a omnipresent, ruthless killer that lurks around dark alleys, silently waiting for naive victims to wander passed so that it might pounce.
I can't get AIDS if I'm in a monogamous relationship the nation's blood supply is safe universal precautions are infallible people with AIDS should be quarantined to protect the rest of society because I don't live in a big city, the threat of AIDS is very small
I know enough about the spread of the disease that I would have no problem working in a health care setting with patients with AIDS the AIDS virus will not ever affect me
Everyone affected with AIDS deserves it due to their lifestyle
Someone with AIDS could be just like me
People infected with AIDS did not have safe sex
Aids affects us all.
People with AIDS should be treated just like everybody else.
AIDS is a disease that anyone can get if there are not careful.
It's easy to get AIDS.
The likelihood of contracting AIDS is very low.
The AIDS quilt is an emotional reminder to remember those who did not deserve to die painfully or in vain
The number of individuals with AIDS in Hollywood is higher than the general public thinks
It is not the AIDS virus that kills people, it is complications from other illnesses (because the immune system isn't functioning) that cause death
AIDS is becoming more a problem for heterosexual women and their offsprings than IV drug users or homosexuals
A cure for AIDS is on the horizon
A cure for AIDS is on the horizon
Mandatory HIV testing should be established for all pregnant women
Rating the Scale Items. OK, so now you have a set of statements. The next step is to have your participants (i.e., judges) rate each statement on a 1-to-11 scale in terms of how much each statement indicates a favorable attitude towards people with AIDS. Pay close attention here! You DON'T want the participants to tell you what their attitudes towards AIDS are, or whether they would agree with the statements. You want them to rate the "favorableness" of each statement in terms of an attitude towards AIDS, where 1 = "extremely unfavorable attitude towards people with AIDS" and 11 = "extremely favorable attitude towards people with AIDS.". (Note that I could just as easily had the judges rate how much each statement represents a negative attitude towards AIDS. If I did, the scale I developed would have higher scale values for people with more negative attitudes).
Computing Scale Score Values for Each Item. The next step is to analyze the rating data. For each statement, you need to compute the Median and the Interquartile Range. The median is the value above and below which 50% of the ratings fall. The first quartile (Q1) is the value below which 25% of the cases fall and above which 75% of the cases fall -- in other words, the 25th percentile. The median is the 50th percentile. The third quartile, Q3, is the 75th percentile. The Interquartile Range is the difference between third and first quartile, or Q3 - Q1. The figure above shows a histogram for a single item and indicates the median and Interquartile Range. You can compute these values easily with any introductory statistics program or with most spreadsheet programs. To facilitate the final selection of items for your scale, you might want to sort the table of medians and Interquartile Range in ascending order by Median and, within that, in descending order by Interquartile Range. For the items in this example, we got a table like the following:
Statement Number
Median
Q1
Q3
Interquartile Range
23
1
1
2.5
1.5
8
1
1
2
1
12
1
1
2
1
34
1
1
2
1
39
1
1
2
1
54
1
1
2
1
56
1
1
2
1
57
1
1
2
1
18
1
1
1
0
25
1
1
1
0
51
1
1
1
0
27
2
1
5
4
45
2
1
4
3
16
2
1
3.5
2.5
42
2
1
3.5
2.5
24
2
1
3
2
44
2
2
4
2
36
2
1
2.5
1.5
43
2
1
2.5
1.5
33
3
1
5
4
48
3
1
5
4
20
3
1.5
5
3.5
28
3
1.5
5
3.5
31
3
1.5
5
3.5
19
3
1
4
3
22
3
1
4
3
37
3
1
4
3
41
3
2
5
3
6
3
1.5
4
2.5
21
3
1.5
4
2.5
32
3
2
4.5
2.5
9
3
2
3.5
1.5
1
4
3
7
4
26
4
1
5
4
47
4
1
5
4
30
4
1.5
5
3.5
13
4
2
5
3
11
4
2
4.5
2.5
15
4
3
5
2
40
5
4.5
8
3.5
2
5
4
6.5
2.5
14
5
4
6
2
17
5.5
4
8
4
49
6
5
9.75
4.75
50
8
5.5
11
5.5
35
8
6.25
10
3.75
29
9
5.5
11
5.5
38
9
5.5
10.5
5
3
9
6
10
4
55
9
7
11
4
10
10
6
10.5
4.5
7
10
7.5
11
3.5
46
10
8
11
3
5
10
8.5
11
2.5
53
11
9.5
11
1.5
4
11
10
11
1
Selecting the Final Scale Items. Now, you have to select the final statements for your scale. You should select statements that are at equal intervals across the range of medians. In our example, we might select one statement for each of the eleven median values. Within each value, you should try to select the statement that has the smallest Interquartile Range. This is the statement with the least amount of variability across judges. You don't want the statistical analysis to be the only deciding factor here. Look over the candidate statements at each level and select the statement that makes the most sense. If you find that the best statistical choice is a confusing statement, select the next best choice.
When we went through our statements, we came up with the following set of items for our scale:
People with AIDS are like my parents (6)
Because AIDS is preventable, we should focus our resources on prevention instead of curing (5)
People with AIDS deserve what they got. (1)
Aids affects us all (10)
People with AIDS should be treated just like everybody else. (11)
AIDS will never happen to me. (3)
It's easy to get AIDS (5)
AIDS doesn't have a preference, anyone can get it (9)
AIDS is a disease that anyone can get if they are not careful (9)
If you have AIDS, you can still lead a normal life (8)
AIDS is good because it helps control the population. (2)
I can't get AIDS if I'm in a monogamous relationship. (4)
The value in parentheses after each statement is its scale value. Items with higher scale values should, in general, indicate a more favorable attitude towards people with AIDS. Notice that we have randomly scrambled the order of the statements with respect to scale values. Also, notice that we do not have an item with scale value of 7 and that we have two with values of 5 and of 9 (one of these pairs will average out to a 7).
Administering the Scale. You now have a scale -- a yardstick you can use for measuring attitudes towards people with AIDS. You can give it to a participant and ask them to agree or disagree with each statement. To get that person's total scale score, you average the scale scores of all the items that person agreed with. For instance, let's say a respondent completed the scale as follows:
Top of Form
Agree
Disagree
People with AIDS are like my parents.
Agree
Disagree
Because AIDS is preventable, we should focus our resources on prevention instead of curing.
Agree
Disagree
People with AIDS deserve what they got.
Agree
Disagree
Aids affects us all.
Agree
Disagree
People with AIDS should be treated just like everybody else.
Agree
Disagree
AIDS will never happen to me.
Agree
Disagree
It's easy to get AIDS.
Agree
Disagree
AIDS doesn't have a preference, anyone can get it.
Agree
Disagree
AIDS is a disease that anyone can get if they are not careful.
Agree
Disagree
If you have AIDS, you can still lead a normal life.
Agree
Disagree
AIDS is good because it helps control the population.
Agree
Disagree
I can't get AIDS if I'm in a monogamous relationship.
Bottom of Form
If you're following along with the example, you should see that the respondent checked eight items as Agree. When we take the average scale values for these eight items, we get a final value for this respondent of 7.75. This is where this particular respondent would fall on our "yardstick" that measures attitudes towards persons with AIDS. Now, let's look at the responses for another individual:
Top of Form
Agree
Disagree
People with AIDS are like my parents.
Agree
Disagree
Because AIDS is preventable, we should focus our resources on prevention instead of curing.
Agree
Disagree
People with AIDS deserve what they got.
Agree
Disagree
Aids affects us all.
Agree
Disagree
People with AIDS should be treated just like everybody else.
Agree
Disagree
AIDS will never happen to me.
Agree
Disagree
It's easy to get AIDS.
Agree
Disagree
AIDS doesn't have a preference, anyone can get it.
Agree
Disagree
AIDS is a disease that anyone can get if they are not careful.
Agree
Disagree
If you have AIDS, you can still lead a normal life.
Agree
Disagree
AIDS is good because it helps control the population.
Agree
Disagree
I can't get AIDS if I'm in a monogamous relationship.
Bottom of Form
In this example, the respondent only checked four items, all of which are on the negative end of the scale. When we average the scale items for the statements with which the respondent agreed we get an average score of 2.5, considerably lower or more negative in attitude than the first respondent.
The Other Thurstone Methods
The other Thurstone scaling methods are similar to the Method of Equal-Appearing Intervals. All of them begin by focusing on a concept that is assumed to be unidimensional and involve generating a large set of potential scale items. All of them result in a scale consisting of relatively few items which the respondent rates on Agree/Disagree basis. The major differences are in how the data from the judges is collected. For instance, the method of paired comparisons requires each judge to make a judgement about each pair of statements. With lots of statements, this can become very time consuming indeed. With 57 statements in the original set, there are 1,596 unique pairs of statements that would have to be compared! Clearly, the paired comparison method would be too time consuming when there are lots of statements initially.
Thurstone methods illustrate well how a simple unidimensional scale might be constructed. There are other approaches, most notably Likert or Summative Scales and Guttman or Cumulative Scales.
Guttman Scaling
Guttman scaling is also sometimes known as cumulative scaling or scalogram analysis. The purpose of Guttman scaling is to establish a one-dimensional continuum for a concept you wish to measure. What does that mean? Essentially, we would like a set of items or statements so that a respondent who agrees with any specific question in the list will also agree with all previous questions. Put more formally, we would like to be able to predict item responses perfectly knowing only the total score for the respondent. For example, imagine a ten-item cumulative scale. If the respondent scores a four, it should mean that he/she agreed with the first four statements. If the respondent scores an eight, it should mean they agreed with the first eight. The object is to find a set of items that perfectly matches this pattern. In practice, we would seldom expect to find this cumulative pattern perfectly. So, we use scalogram analysis to examine how closely a set of items corresponds with this idea of cumulativeness. Here, I'll explain how we develop a Guttman scale.
Define the Focus. As in all of the scaling methods. we begin by defining the focus for our scale. Let's imagine that you wish to develop a cumulative scale that measures U.S. citizen attitudes towards immigration. You would want to be sure to specify in your definition whether you are talking about any type of immigration (legal and illegal) from anywhere (Europe, Asia, Latin and South America, Africa).
Develop the Items. Next, as in all scaling methods, you would develop a large set of items that reflect the concept. You might do this yourself or you might engage a knowledgeable group to help. Let's say you came up with the following statements:
I would permit a child of mine to marry an immigrant.
I believe that this country should allow more immigrants in.
I would be comfortable if a new immigrant moved next door to me.
I would be comfortable with new immigrants moving into my community.
It would be fine with me if new immigrants moved onto my block.
I would be comfortable if my child dated a new immigrant.
Of course, we would want to come up with many more statements (about 80-100 would be desirable).
Rate the Items. Next, we would want to have a group of judges rate the statements or items in terms of how favorable they are to the concept of immigration. They would give a Yes if the item was favorable toward immigration and a No if it is not. Notice that we are not asking the judges whether they personally agree with the statement. Instead, we're asking them to make a judgment about how the statement is related to the construct of interest.
Develop the Cumulative Scale. The key to Guttman scaling is in the analysis. We construct a matrix or table that shows the responses of all the respondents on all of the items. We then sort this matrix so that respondents who agree with more statements are listed at the top and those agreeing with fewer are at the bottom. For respondents with the same number of agreements, we sort the statements from left to right from those that most agreed to to those that fewest agreed to. We might get a table something like the figure. Notice that the scale is very nearly cumulative when you read from left to right across the columns (items). Specifically if someone agreed with Item 7, they always agreed with Item 2. And, if someone agreed with Item 5, they always agreed with Items 7 and 2. The matrix shows that the cumulativeness of the scale is not perfect, however. While in general, a person agreeing with Item 3 tended to also agree with 5, 7 and 2, there are several exceptions to that rule.
While we can examine the matrix if there are only a few items in it, if there are lots of items, we need to use a data analysis called scalogram analysis to determine the subsets of items from our pool that best approximate the cumulative property. Then, we review these items and select our final scale elements. There are several statistical techniques for examining the table to find a cumulative scale. Because there is seldom a perfectly cumulative scale we usually have to test how good it is. These statistics also estimate a scale score value for each item. This scale score is used in the final calculation of a respondent's score.
Administering the Scale. Once you've selected the final scale items, it's relatively simple to administer the scale. You simply present the items and ask the respondent to check items with which they agree. For our hypothetical immigration scale, the items might be listed in cumulative order as:
I believe that this country should allow more immigrants in.
I would be comfortable with new immigrants moving into my community.
It would be fine with me if new immigrants moved onto my block.
I would be comfortable if a new immigrant moved next door to me.
I would be comfortable if my child dated a new immigrant.
I would permit a child of mine to marry an immigrant.
Of course, when we give the items to the respondent, we would probably want to mix up the order. Our final scale might look like:
INSTRUCTIONS: Place a check next to each statement you agree with.
_____ I would permit a child of mine to marry an immigrant.
_____ I believe that this country should allow more immigrants in.
_____ I would be comfortable if a new immigrant moved next door to me.
_____ I would be comfortable with new immigrants moving into my community.
_____ It would be fine with me if new immigrants moved onto my block.
_____ I would be comfortable if my child dated a new immigrant.
Each scale item has a scale value associated with it (obtained from the scalogram analysis). To compute a respondent's scale score we simply sum the scale values of every item they agree with. In our example, their final value should be an indication of their attitude towards immigration.
Sampling
Sampling is the process of selecting units (e.g., people, organizations) from a population of interest so that by studying the sample we may fairly generalize our results back to the population from which they were chosen. Let's begin by covering some of the key terms in sampling like "population" and "sampling frame." Then, because some types of sampling rely upon quantitative models, we'll talk about some of the statistical terms used in sampling. Finally, we'll discuss the major distinction between probability and Nonprobability sampling methods and work through the major types in each.
External Validity
External validity is related to generalizing. That's the major thing you need to keep in mind. Recall that validity refers to the approximate truth of propositions, inferences, or conclusions. So, external validity refers to the approximate truth of conclusions the involve generalizations. Put in more pedestrian terms, external validity is the degree to which the conclusions in your study would hold for other persons in other places and at other times.
In science there are two major approaches to how we provide evidence for a generalization. I'll call the first approach the Sampling Model. In the sampling model, you start by identifying the population you would like to generalize to. Then, you draw a fair sample from that population and conduct your research with the sample. Finally, because the sample is representative of the population, you can automatically generalize your results back to the population. There are several problems with this approach. First, perhaps you don't know at the time of your study who you might ultimately like to generalize to. Second, you may not be easily able to draw a fair or representative sample. Third, it's impossible to sample across all times that you might like to generalize to (like next year).
I'll call the second approach to generalizing the Proximal Similarity Model. 'Proximal' means 'nearby' and 'similarity' means... well, it means 'similarity'. The term proximal similarity was suggested by Donald T. Campbell as an appropriate relabeling of the term external validity (although he was the first to admit that it probably wouldn't catch on!). Under this model, we begin by thinking about different generalizability contexts and developing a theory about which contexts are more like our study and which are less so. For instance, we might imagine several settings that have people who are more similar to the people in our study or people who are less similar. This also holds for times and places. When we place different contexts in terms of their relative similarities, we can call this implicit theoretical a gradient of similarity. Once we have developed this proximal similarity framework, we are able to generalize. How? We conclude that we can generalize the results of our study to other persons, places or times that are more like (that is, more proximally similar) to our study. Notice that here, we can never generalize with certainty -- it is always a question of more or less similar.
Threats to External Validity
A threat to external validity is an explanation of how you might be wrong in making a generalization. For instance, you conclude that the results of your study (which was done in a specific place, with certain types of people, and at a specific time) can be generalized to another context (for instance, another place, with slightly different people, at a slightly later time). There are three major threats to external validity because there are three ways you could be wrong -- people, places or times. Your critics could come along, for example, and argue that the results of your study are due to the unusual type of people who were in the study. Or, they could argue that it might only work because of the unusual place you did the study in (perhaps you did your educational study in a college town with lots of high-achieving educationally-oriented kids). Or, they might suggest that you did your study in a peculiar time. For instance, if you did your smoking cessation study the week after the Surgeon General issues the well-publicized results of the latest smoking and cancer studies, you might get different results than if you had done it the week before.
Improving External Validity
How can we improve external validity? One way, based on the sampling model, suggests that you do a good job of drawing a sample from a population. For instance, you should use random selection, if possible, rather than a nonrandom procedure. And, once selected, you should try to assure that the respondents participate in your study and that you keep your dropout rates low. A second approach would be to use the theory of proximal similarity more effectively. How? Perhaps you could do a better job of describing the ways your contexts and others differ, providing lots of data about the degree of similarity between various groups of people, places, and even times. You might even be able to map out the degree of proximal similarity among various contexts with a methodology like concept mapping. Perhaps the best approach to criticisms of generalizations is simply to show them that they're wrong -- do your study in a variety of places, with different people and at different times. That is, your external validity (ability to generalize) will be stronger the more you replicate your study.
Sampling Terminology
As with anything else in life you have to learn the language of an area if you're going to ever hope to use it. Here, I want to introduce several different terms for the major groups that are involved in a sampling process and the role that each group plays in the logic of sampling.
The major question that motivates sampling in the first place is: "Who do you want to generalize to?" Or should it be: "To whom do you want to generalize?" In most social research we are interested in more than just the people who directly participate in our study. We would like to be able to talk in general terms and not be confined only to the people who are in our study. Now, there are times when we aren't very concerned about generalizing. Maybe we're just evaluating a program in a local agency and we don't care whether the program would work with other people in other places and at other times. In that case, sampling and generalizing might not be of interest. In other cases, we would really like to be able to generalize almost universally. When psychologists do research, they are often interested in developing theories that would hold for all humans. But in most applied social research, we are interested in generalizing to specific groups. The group you wish to generalize to is often called the population in your study. This is the group you would like to sample from because this is the group you are interested in generalizing to. Let's imagine that you wish to generalize to urban homeless males between the ages of 30 and 50 in the United States. If that is the population of interest, you are likely to have a very hard time developing a reasonable sampling plan. You are probably not going to find an accurate listing of this population, and even if you did, you would almost certainly not be able to mount a national sample across hundreds of urban areas. So we probably should make a distinction between the population you would like to generalize to, and the population that will be accessible to you. We'll call the former the theoretical population and the latter the accessible population. In this example, the accessible population might be homeless males between the ages of 30 and 50 in six selected urban areas across the U.S.
Once you've identified the theoretical and accessible populations, you have to do one more thing before you can actually draw a sample -- you have to get a list of the members of the accessible population. (Or, you have to spell out in detail how you will contact them to assure representativeness). The listing of the accessible population from which you'll draw your sample is called the sampling frame. If you were doing a phone survey and selecting names from the telephone book, the book would be your sampling frame. That wouldn't be a great way to sample because significant subportions of the population either don't have a phone or have moved in or out of the area since the last book was printed. Notice that in this case, you might identify the area code and all three-digit prefixes within that area code and draw a sample simply by randomly dialing numbers (cleverly known as random-digit-dialing). In this case, the sampling frame is not a list per se, but is rather a procedure that you follow as the actual basis for sampling. Finally, you actually draw your sample (using one of the many sampling procedures). The sample is the group of people who you select to be in your study. Notice that I didn't say that the sample was the group of people who are actually in your study. You may not be able to contact or recruit all of the people you actually sample, or some could drop out over the course of the study. The group that actually completes your study is a subsample of the sample -- it doesn't include nonrespondents or dropouts. The problem of nonresponse and its effects on a study will be addressed when discussing "mortality" threats to internal validity.
People often confuse what is meant by random selection with the idea of random assignment. You should make sure that you understand the distinction between random selection and random assignment.
At this point, you should appreciate that sampling is a difficult multi-step process and that there are lots of places you can go wrong. In fact, as we move from each step to the next in identifying a sample, there is the possibility of introducing systematic error or bias. For instance, even if you are able to identify perfectly the population of interest, you may not have access to all of them. And even if you do, you may not have a complete and accurate enumeration or sampling frame from which to select. And, even if you do, you may not draw the sample correctly or accurately. And, even if you do, they may not all come and they may not all stay. Depressed yet? This is a very difficult business indeed. At times like this I'm reminded of what Donald Campbell used to say (I'll paraphrase here): "Cousins to the amoeba, it's amazing that we know anything at all!"
Statistical Terms in Sampling
Let's begin by defining some very simple terms that are relevant here. First, let's look at the results of our sampling efforts. When we sample, the units that we sample -- usually people -- supply us with one or more responses. In this sense, a response is a specific measurement value that a sampling unit supplies. In the figure, the person is responding to a survey instrument and gives a response of '4'. When we look across the responses that we get for our entire sample, we use a statistic. There are a wide variety of statistics we can use -- mean, median, mode, and so on. In this example, we see that the mean or average for the sample is 3.75. But the reason we sample is so that we might get an estimate for the population we sampled from. If we could, we would much prefer to measure the entire population. If you measure the entire population and calculate a value like a mean or average, we don't refer to this as a statistic, we call it a parameter of the population.
The Sampling Distribution
So how do we get from our sample statistic to an estimate of the population parameter? A crucial midway concept you need to understand is the sampling distribution. In order to understand it, you have to be able and willing to do a thought experiment. Imagine that instead of just taking a single sample like we do in a typical study, you took three independent samples of the same population. And furthermore, imagine that for each of your three samples, you collected a single response and computed a single statistic, say, the mean of the response. Even though all three samples came from the same population, you wouldn't expect to get the exact same statistic from each. They would differ slightly just due to the random "luck of the draw" or to the natural fluctuations or vagaries of drawing a sample. But you would expect that all three samples would yield a similar statistical estimate because they were drawn from the same population. Now, for the leap of imagination! Imagine that you did an infinite number of samples from the same population and computed the average for each one. If you plotted them on a histogram or bar graph you should find that most of them converge on the same central value and that you get fewer and fewer samples that have averages farther away up or down from that central value. In other words, the bar graph would be well described by the bell curve shape that is an indication of a "normal" distribution in statistics. The distribution of an infinite number of samples of the same size as the sample in your study is known as the sampling distribution. We don't ever actually construct a sampling distribution. Why not? You're not paying attention! Because to construct it we would have to take an infinite number of samples and at least the last time I checked, on this planet infinite is not a number we know how to reach. So why do we even talk about a sampling distribution? Now that's a good question! Because we need to realize that our sample is just one of a potentially infinite number of samples that we could have taken. When we keep the sampling distribution in mind, we realize that while the statistic we got from our sample is probably near the center of the sampling distribution (because most of the samples would be there) we could have gotten one of the extreme samples just by the luck of the draw. If we take the average of the sampling distribution -- the average of the averages of an infinite number of samples -- we would be much closer to the true population average -- the parameter of interest. So the average of the sampling distribution is essentially equivalent to the parameter. But what is the standard deviation of the sampling distribution (OK, never had statistics? There are any number of places on the web where you can learn about them or even just brush up if you've gotten rusty. This isn't one of them. I'm going to assume that you at least know what a standard deviation is, or that you're capable of finding out relatively quickly). The standard deviation of the sampling distribution tells us something about how different samples would be distributed. In statistics it is referred to as the standard error (so we can keep it separate in our minds from standard deviations. Getting confused? Go get a cup of coffee and come back in ten minutes...OK, let's try once more... A standard deviation is the spread of the scores around the average in a single sample. The standard error is the spread of the averages around the average of averages in a sampling distribution. Got it?)
Sampling Error
In sampling contexts, the standard error is called sampling error. Sampling error gives us some idea of the precision of our statistical estimate. A low sampling error means that we had relatively less variability or range in the sampling distribution. But here we go again -- we never actually see the sampling distribution! So how do we calculate sampling error? We base our calculation on the standard deviation of our sample. The greater the sample standard deviation, the greater the standard error (and the sampling error). The standard error is also related to the sample size. The greater your sample size, the smaller the standard error. Why? Because the greater the sample size, the closer your sample is to the actual population itself. If you take a sample that consists of the entire population you actually have no sampling error because you don't have a sample, you have the entire population. In that case, the mean you estimate is the parameter.
The 68, 95, 99 Percent Rule
You've probably heard this one before, but it's so important that it's always worth repeating... There is a general rule that applies whenever we have a normal or bell-shaped distribution. Start with the average -- the center of the distribution. If you go up and down (i.e., left and right) one standard unit, you will include approximately 68% of the cases in the distribution (i.e., 68% of the area under the curve). If you go up and down two standard units, you will include approximately 95% of the cases. And if you go plus-and-minus three standard units, you will include about 99% of the cases. Notice that I didn't specify in the previous few sentences whether I was talking about standard deviation units or standard error units. That's because the same rule holds for both types of distributions (i.e., the raw data and sampling distributions). For instance, in the figure, the mean of the distribution is 3.75 and the standard unit is .25 (If this was a distribution of raw data, we would be talking in standard deviation units. If it's a sampling distribution, we'd be talking in standard error units). If we go up and down one standard unit from the mean, we would be going up and down .25 from the mean of 3.75. Within this range -- 3.5 to 4.0 -- we would expect to see approximately 68% of the cases. This section is marked in red on the figure. I leave to you to figure out the other ranges. But what does this all mean you ask? If we are dealing with raw data and we know the mean and standard deviation of a sample, we can predict the intervals within which 68, 95 and 99% of our cases would be expected to fall. We call these intervals the -- guess what -- 68, 95 and 99% confidence intervals.
Now, here's where everything should come together in one great aha! experience if you've been following along. If we had a sampling distribution, we would be able to predict the 68, 95 and 99% confidence intervals for where the population parameter should be! And isn't that why we sampled in the first place? So that we could predict where the population is on that variable? There's only one hitch. We don't actually have the sampling distribution (now this is the third time I've said this in this essay)! But we do have the distribution for the sample itself. And we can from that distribution estimate the standard error (the sampling error) because it is based on the standard deviation and we have that. And, of course, we don't actually know the population parameter value -- we're trying to find that out -- but we can use our best estimate for that -- the sample statistic. Now, if we have the mean of the sampling distribution (or set it to the mean from our sample) and we have an estimate of the standard error (we calculate that from our sample) then we have the two key ingredients that we need for our sampling distribution in order to estimate confidence intervals for the population parameter.
Perhaps an example will help. Let's assume we did a study and drew a single sample from the population. Furthermore, let's assume that the average for the sample was 3.75 and the standard deviation was .25. This is the raw data distribution depicted above. now, what would the sampling distribution be in this case? Well, we don't actually construct it (because we would need to take an infinite number of samples) but we can estimate it. For starters, we assume that the mean of the sampling distribution is the mean of the sample, which is 3.75. Then, we calculate the standard error. To do this, we use the standard deviation for our sample and the sample size (in this case N=100) and we come up with a standard error of .025 (just trust me on this). Now we have everything we need to estimate a confidence interval for the population parameter. We would estimate that the probability is 68% that the true parameter value falls between 3.725 and 3.775 (i.e., 3.75 plus and minus .025); that the 95% confidence interval is 3.700 to 3.800; and that we can say with 99% confidence that the population value is between 3.675 and 3.825. The real value (in this fictitious example) was 3.72 and so we have correctly estimated that value with our sample.
Probability Sampling
A probability sampling method is any method of sampling that utilizes some form of random selection. In order to have a random selection method, you must set up some process or procedure that assures that the different units in your population have equal probabilities of being chosen. Humans have long practiced various forms of random selection, such as picking a name out of a hat, or choosing the short straw. These days, we tend to use computers as the mechanism for generating random numbers as the basis for random selection.
Some Definitions
Before I can explain the various probability methods we have to define some basic terms. These are:
N = the number of cases in the sampling frame n = the number of cases in the sample
NCn = the number of combinations (subsets) of n from N f = n/N = the sampling fraction
That's it. With those terms defined we can begin to define the different probability sampling methods.
Simple Random Sampling
The simplest form of random sampling is called simple random sampling. Pretty tricky, huh? Here's the quick description of simple random sampling:
Objective: To select n units out of N such that each NCn has an equal chance of being selected.
Procedure: Use a table of random numbers, a computer random number generator, or a mechanical device to select the sample.
A somewhat stilted, if accurate, definition. Let's see if we can make it a little more real. How do we select a simple random sample? Let's assume that we are doing some research with a small service agency that wishes to assess client's views of quality of service over the past year. First, we have to get the sampling frame organized. To accomplish this, we'll go through agency records to identify every client over the past 12 months. If we're lucky, the agency has good accurate computerized records and can quickly produce such a list. Then, we have to actually draw the sample. Decide on the number of clients you would like to have in the final sample. For the sake of the example, let's say you want to select 100 clients to survey and that there were 1000 clients over the past 12 months. Then, the sampling fraction is f = n/N = 100/1000 = .10 or 10%. Now, to actually draw the sample, you have several options. You could print off the list of 1000 clients, tear then into separate strips, put the strips in a hat, mix them up real good, close your eyes and pull out the first 100. But this mechanical procedure would be tedious and the quality of the sample would depend on how thoroughly you mixed them up and how randomly you reached in. Perhaps a better procedure would be to use the kind of ball machine that is popular with many of the state lotteries. You would need three sets of balls numbered 0 to 9, one set for each of the digits from 000 to 999 (if we select 000 we'll call that 1000). Number the list of names from 1 to 1000 and then use the ball machine to select the three digits that selects each person. The obvious disadvantage here is that you need to get the ball machines. (Where do they make those things, anyway? Is there a ball machine industry?).
Neither of these mechanical procedures is very feasible and, with the development of inexpensive computers there is a much easier way. Here's a simple procedure that's especially useful if you have the names of the clients already on the computer. Many computer programs can generate a series of random numbers. Let's assume you can copy and paste the list of client names into a column in an EXCEL spreadsheet. Then, in the column right next to it paste the function =RAND() which is EXCEL's way of putting a random number between 0 and 1 in the cells. Then, sort both columns -- the list of names and the random number -- by the random numbers. This rearranges the list in random order from the lowest to the highest random number. Then, all you have to do is take the first hundred names in this sorted list. pretty simple. You could probably accomplish the whole thing in under a minute.
Simple random sampling is simple to accomplish and is easy to explain to others. Because simple random sampling is a fair way to select a sample, it is reasonable to generalize the results from the sample back to the population. Simple random sampling is not the most statistically efficient method of sampling and you may, just because of the luck of the draw, not get good representation of subgroups in a population. To deal with these issues, we have to turn to other sampling methods.
Stratified Random Sampling
Stratified Random Sampling, also sometimes called proportional or quota random sampling, involves dividing your population into homogeneous subgroups and then taking a simple random sample in each subgroup. In more formal terms:
Objective: Divide the population into non-overlapping groups (i.e., strata) N1, N2, N3, ... Ni, such that N1 + N2 + N3 + ... + Ni = N. Then do a simple random sample of f = n/N in each strata.
There are several major reasons why you might prefer stratified sampling over simple random sampling. First, it assures that you will be able to represent not only the overall population, but also key subgroups of the population, especially small minority groups. If you want to be able to talk about subgroups, this may be the only way to effectively assure you'll be able to. If the subgroup is extremely small, you can use different sampling fractions (f) within the different strata to randomly over-sample the small group (although you'll then have to weight the within-group estimates using the sampling fraction whenever you want overall population estimates). When we use the same sampling fraction within strata we are conducting proportionate stratified random sampling. When we use different sampling fractions in the strata, we call this disproportionate stratified random sampling. Second, stratified random sampling will generally have more statistical precision than simple random sampling. This will only be true if the strata or groups are homogeneous. If they are, we expect that the variability within-groups is lower than the variability for the population as a whole. Stratified sampling capitalizes on that fact.
For example, let's say that the population of clients for our agency can be divided into three groups: Caucasian, African-American and Hispanic-American. Furthermore, let's assume that both the African-Americans and Hispanic-Americans are relatively small minorities of the clientele (10% and 5% respectively). If we just did a simple random sample of n=100 with a sampling fraction of 10%, we would expect by chance alone that we would only get 10 and 5 persons from each of our two smaller groups. And, by chance, we could get fewer than that! If we stratify, we can do better. First, let's determine how many people we want to have in each group. Let's say we still want to take a sample of 100 from the population of 1000 clients over the past year. But we think that in order to say anything about subgroups we will need at least 25 cases in each group. So, let's sample 50 Caucasians, 25 African-Americans, and 25 Hispanic-Americans. We know that 10% of the population, or 100 clients, are African-American. If we randomly sample 25 of these, we have a within-stratum sampling fraction of 25/100 = 25%. Similarly, we know that 5% or 50 clients are Hispanic-American. So our within-stratum sampling fraction will be 25/50 = 50%. Finally, by subtraction we know that there are 850 Caucasian clients. Our within-stratum sampling fraction for them is 50/850 = about 5.88%. Because the groups are more homogeneous within-group than across the population as a whole, we can expect greater statistical precision (less variance). And, because we stratified, we know we will have enough cases from each group to make meaningful subgroup inferences.
Systematic Random Sampling
Here are the steps you need to follow in order to achieve a systematic random sample: number the units in the population from 1 to N decide on the n (sample size) that you want or need k = N/n = the interval size randomly select an integer between 1 to k then take every kth unit
All of this will be much clearer with an example. Let's assume that we have a population that only has N=100 people in it and that you want to take a sample of n=20. To use systematic sampling, the population must be listed in a random order. The sampling fraction would be f = 20/100 = 20%. in this case, the interval size, k, is equal to N/n = 100/20 = 5. Now, select a random integer from 1 to 5. In our example, imagine that you chose 4. Now, to select the sample, start with the 4th unit in the list and take every k-th unit (every 5th, because k=5). You would be sampling units 4, 9, 14, 19, and so on to 100 and you would wind up with 20 units in your sample.
For this to work, it is essential that the units in the population are randomly ordered, at least with respect to the characteristics you are measuring. Why would you ever want to use systematic random sampling? For one thing, it is fairly easy to do. You only have to select a single random number to start things off. It may also be more precise than simple random sampling. Finally, in some situations there is simply no easier way to do random sampling. For instance, I once had to do a study that involved sampling from all the books in a library. Once selected, I would have to go to the shelf, locate the book, and record when it last circulated. I knew that I had a fairly good sampling frame in the form of the shelf list (which is a card catalog where the entries are arranged in the order they occur on the shelf). To do a simple random sample, I could have estimated the total number of books and generated random numbers to draw the sample; but how would I find book #74,329 easily if that is the number I selected? I couldn't very well count the cards until I came to 74,329! Stratifying wouldn't solve that problem either. For instance, I could have stratified by card catalog drawer and drawn a simple random sample within each drawer. But I'd still be stuck counting cards. Instead, I did a systematic random sample. I estimated the number of books in the entire collection. Let's imagine it was 100,000. I decided that I wanted to take a sample of 1000 for a sampling fraction of 1000/100,000 = 1%. To get the sampling interval k, I divided N/n = 100,000/1000 = 100. Then I selected a random integer between 1 and 100. Let's say I got 57. Next I did a little side study to determine how thick a thousand cards are in the card catalog (taking into account the varying ages of the cards). Let's say that on average I found that two cards that were separated by 100 cards were about .75 inches apart in the catalog drawer. That information gave me everything I needed to draw the sample. I counted to the 57th by hand and recorded the book information. Then, I took a compass. (Remember those from your high-school math class? They're the funny little metal instruments with a sharp pin on one end and a pencil on the other that you used to draw circles in geometry class.) Then I set the compass at .75", stuck the pin end in at the 57th card and pointed with the pencil end to the next card (approximately 100 books away). In this way, I approximated selecting the 157th, 257th, 357th, and so on. I was able to accomplish the entire selection procedure in very little time using this systematic random sampling approach. I'd probably still be there counting cards if I'd tried another random sampling method. (Okay, so I have no life. I got compensated nicely, I don't mind saying, for coming up with this scheme.)
Cluster (Area) Random Sampling
The problem with random sampling methods when we have to sample a population that's disbursed across a wide geographic region is that you will have to cover a lot of ground geographically in order to get to each of the units you sampled. Imagine taking a simple random sample of all the residents of New York State in order to conduct personal interviews. By the luck of the draw you will wind up with respondents who come from all over the state. Your interviewers are going to have a lot of traveling to do. It is for precisely this problem that cluster or area random sampling was invented.
In cluster sampling, we follow these steps: divide population into clusters (usually along geographic boundaries) randomly sample clusters measure all units within sampled clusters
For instance, in the figure we see a map of the counties in New York State. Let's say that we have to do a survey of town governments that will require us going to the towns personally. If we do a simple random sample state-wide we'll have to cover the entire state geographically. Instead, we decide to do a cluster sampling of five counties (marked in red in the figure). Once these are selected, we go to every town government in the five areas. Clearly this strategy will help us to economize on our mileage. Cluster or area sampling, then, is useful in situations like this, and is done primarily for efficiency of administration. Note also, that we probably don't have to worry about using this approach if we are conducting a mail or telephone survey because it doesn't matter as much (or cost more or raise inefficiency) where we call or send letters to.
Multi-Stage Sampling
The four methods we've covered so far -- simple, stratified, systematic and cluster -- are the simplest random sampling strategies. In most real applied social research, we would use sampling methods that are considerably more complex than these simple variations. The most important principle here is that we can combine the simple methods described earlier in a variety of useful ways that help us address our sampling needs in the most efficient and effective manner possible. When we combine sampling methods, we call this multi-stage sampling.
For example, consider the idea of sampling New York State residents for face-to-face interviews. Clearly we would want to do some type of cluster sampling as the first stage of the process. We might sample townships or census tracts throughout the state. But in cluster sampling we would then go on to measure everyone in the clusters we select. Even if we are sampling census tracts we may not be able to measure everyone who is in the census tract. So, we might set up a stratified sampling process within the clusters. In this case, we would have a two-stage sampling process with stratified samples within cluster samples. Or, consider the problem of sampling students in grade schools. We might begin with a national sample of school districts stratified by economics and educational level. Within selected districts, we might do a simple random sample of schools. Within schools, we might do a simple random sample of classes or grades. And, within classes, we might even do a simple random sample of students. In this case, we have three or four stages in the sampling process and we use both stratified and simple random sampling. By combining different sampling methods we are able to achieve a rich variety of probabilistic sampling methods that can be used in a wide range of social research contexts.
Nonprobability Sampling
The difference between nonprobability and probability sampling is that nonprobability sampling does not involve random selection and probability sampling does. Does that mean that nonprobability samples aren't representative of the population? Not necessarily. But it does mean that nonprobability samples cannot depend upon the rationale of probability theory. At least with a probabilistic sample, we know the odds or probability that we have represented the population well. We are able to estimate confidence intervals for the statistic. With nonprobability samples, we may or may not represent the population well, and it will often be hard for us to know how well we've done so. In general, researchers prefer probabilistic or random sampling methods over nonprobabilistic ones, and consider them to be more accurate and rigorous. However, in applied social research there may be circumstances where it is not feasible, practical or theoretically sensible to do random sampling. Here, we consider a wide range of nonprobabilistic alternatives.
We can divide nonprobability sampling methods into two broad types: accidental or purposive. Most sampling methods are purposive in nature because we usually approach the sampling problem with a specific plan in mind. The most important distinctions among these types of sampling methods are the ones between the different types of purposive sampling approaches.
Accidental, Haphazard or Convenience Sampling
One of the most common methods of sampling goes under the various titles listed here. I would include in this category the traditional "man on the street" (of course, now it's probably the "person on the street") interviews conducted frequently by television news programs to get a quick (although nonrepresentative) reading of public opinion. I would also argue that the typical use of college students in much psychological research is primarily a matter of convenience. (You don't really believe that psychologists use college students because they believe they're representative of the population at large, do you?). In clinical practice,we might use clients who are available to us as our sample. In many research contexts, we sample simply by asking for volunteers. Clearly, the problem with all of these types of samples is that we have no evidence that they are representative of the populations we're interested in generalizing to -- and in many cases we would clearly suspect that they are not.
Purposive Sampling
In purposive sampling, we sample with a purpose in mind. We usually would have one or more specific predefined groups we are seeking. For instance, have you ever run into people in a mall or on the street who are carrying a clipboard and who are stopping various people and asking if they could interview them? Most likely they are conducting a purposive sample (and most likely they are engaged in market research). They might be looking for Caucasian females between 30-40 years old. They size up the people passing by and anyone who looks to be in that category they stop to ask if they will participate. One of the first things they're likely to do is verify that the respondent does in fact meet the criteria for being in the sample. Purposive sampling can be very useful for situations where you need to reach a targeted sample quickly and where sampling for proportionality is not the primary concern. With a purposive sample, you are likely to get the opinions of your target population, but you are also likely to overweight subgroups in your population that are more readily accessible.
All of the methods that follow can be considered subcategories of purposive sampling methods. We might sample for specific groups or types of people as in modal instance, expert, or quota sampling. We might sample for diversity as in heterogeneity sampling. Or, we might capitalize on informal social networks to identify specific respondents who are hard to locate otherwise, as in snowball sampling. In all of these methods we know what we want -- we are sampling with a purpose.
Modal Instance Sampling
In statistics, the mode is the most frequently occurring value in a distribution. In sampling, when we do a modal instance sample, we are sampling the most frequent case, or the "typical" case. In a lot of informal public opinion polls, for instance, they interview a "typical" voter. There are a number of problems with this sampling approach. First, how do we know what the "typical" or "modal" case is? We could say that the modal voter is a person who is of average age, educational level, and income in the population. But, it's not clear that using the averages of these is the fairest (consider the skewed distribution of income, for instance). And, how do you know that those three variables -- age, education, income -- are the only or even the most relevant for classifying the typical voter? What if religion or ethnicity is an important discriminator? Clearly, modal instance sampling is only sensible for informal sampling contexts.
Expert Sampling
Expert sampling involves the assembling of a sample of persons with known or demonstrable experience and expertise in some area. Often, we convene such a sample under the auspices of a "panel of experts." There are actually two reasons you might do expert sampling. First, because it would be the best way to elicit the views of persons who have specific expertise. In this case, expert sampling is essentially just a specific subcase of purposive sampling. But the other reason you might use expert sampling is to provide evidence for the validity of another sampling approach you've chosen. For instance, let's say you do modal instance sampling and are concerned that the criteria you used for defining the modal instance are subject to criticism. You might convene an expert panel consisting of persons with acknowledged experience and insight into that field or topic and ask them to examine your modal definitions and comment on their appropriateness and validity. The advantage of doing this is that you aren't out on your own trying to defend your decisions -- you have some acknowledged experts to back you. The disadvantage is that even the experts can be, and often are, wrong.
Quota Sampling
In quota sampling, you select people nonrandomly according to some fixed quota. There are two types of quota sampling: proportional and non proportional. In proportional quota sampling you want to represent the major characteristics of the population by sampling a proportional amount of each. For instance, if you know the population has 40% women and 60% men, and that you want a total sample size of 100, you will continue sampling until you get those percentages and then you will stop. So, if you've already got the 40 women for your sample, but not the sixty men, you will continue to sample men but even if legitimate women respondents come along, you will not sample them because you have already "met your quota." The problem here (as in much purposive sampling) is that you have to decide the specific characteristics on which you will base the quota. Will it be by gender, age, education race, religion, etc.?
Nonproportional quota sampling is a bit less restrictive. In this method, you specify the minimum number of sampled units you want in each category. here, you're not concerned with having numbers that match the proportions in the population. Instead, you simply want to have enough to assure that you will be able to talk about even small groups in the population. This method is the nonprobabilistic analogue of stratified random sampling in that it is typically used to assure that smaller groups are adequately represented in your sample.
Heterogeneity Sampling
We sample for heterogeneity when we want to include all opinions or views, and we aren't concerned about representing these views proportionately. Another term for this is sampling for diversity. In many brainstorming or nominal group processes (including concept mapping), we would use some form of heterogeneity sampling because our primary interest is in getting broad spectrum of ideas, not identifying the "average" or "modal instance" ones. In effect, what we would like to be sampling is not people, but ideas. We imagine that there is a universe of all possible ideas relevant to some topic and that we want to sample this population, not the population of people who have the ideas. Clearly, in order to get all of the ideas, and especially the "outlier" or unusual ones, we have to include a broad and diverse range of participants. Heterogeneity sampling is, in this sense, almost the opposite of modal instance sampling.
Snowball Sampling
In snowball sampling, you begin by identifying someone who meets the criteria for inclusion in your study. You then ask them to recommend others who they may know who also meet the criteria. Although this method would hardly lead to representative samples, there are times when it may be the best method available. Snowball sampling is especially useful when you are trying to reach populations that are inaccessible or hard to find. For instance, if you are studying the homeless, you are not likely to be able to find good lists of homeless people within a specific geographical area. However, if you go to that area and identify one or two, you may find that they know very well who the other homeless people in their vicinity are and how you can find them.
You May Also Find These Documents Helpful
-
During the 1600’s many ideas and values affected the political, economic, and social development of the New England colonies (specifically from 1630 through the 1660’s) . The puritains had a close kinship, for example “working as one man”, and entertaining eachother in brotherly affection. The Puritans were a significant grouping of English Protestants in the 16th and 17th centuries. Puritanism in this sense was founded by some Marian exiles from the clergy shortly after the accession of Elizabeth I of England in 1558, as an activist movement within the Church of England. The designation "Puritan" is often incorrectly used, notably based on the assumption that hedonism and Puritanism are antonyms: Historically, the word was used to characterize the Protestant group as extremists similar to the Cathari of France, and according to Thomas Fuller in his Church History dated back to 1564, Archbishop Matthew Parker of that time used it and "precisian" with the sense of modern "stickler".…
- 586 Words
- 3 Pages
Good Essays -
Questionnaires are written questions; these questions are given out to a number of respondents for a researcher to find out certain information. When the researcher sends out a questionnaire each one is written in the same order so that it is easier for the information to be collected and collated. Once the results have been collected and collated they can be analysed using computers and you can also use large samples and surveys. They are made for a purpose of gathering information for a survey or a statistical study. Most of the time questionnaires use quantitative data which means numbers and statistics are mainly used. This is positive because when we have numbers and statistics we are able to see different trends and patterns which is really helpful when trying to find out particular results. The most common type of questionnaire used for sociological research would be the self- completion questionnaire where it is given out for people to complete. When structured interviews are taken place it is normally by a researcher that is highly trained and this means that they guide the respondent through the question preventing any errors or misunderstandings that could be…
- 1272 Words
- 6 Pages
Powerful Essays -
Survey research is the process of interviewing participants or administering questionnaires and written surveys to a large number of people. (CliffsNotes). The data is then studied to determine trends, differences and similarities. Predications are then made about the participants. The advantage to survey research is inexpensive data acquisition, convenience to participants schedule and larger volume of participant information. However, it does include disadvantages as well. There can be volunteer or interviewer bias and distortion. “Distortion occurs when a subject does not respond to questions honestly”. (CliffNotes). Volunteer bias is not having…
- 943 Words
- 4 Pages
Good Essays -
The participants were each asked to fill a questionnaire that has been prepared specifically for this research. Some participants preferred to fill the questionnaire before sampling, some chose to do so after sampling, while a few others were orally asked the questions while sampling was been carried out. It was mandatory that each questionnaire was filled in my presence, so that no important questions were misunderstood or left unanswered.…
- 940 Words
- 4 Pages
Powerful Essays -
Survey research is one of the most important areas of measurement in social research. It involves asking questions to various respondents and come up with a conclusion. Some examples of what surveys can be used for is to find out how many people have a chronic disease, if it’s in the early stages or more developed, if they’re happy with the services they’ve received, and if they feel like the costs of the services rendered are worth the money. Not only can surveys be used for the patients opinions, but they can also be used to survey doctors and health care…
- 946 Words
- 4 Pages
Good Essays -
15. Interviewing- allows for more information from an individual than survey, which aggregates group responses.…
- 386 Words
- 2 Pages
Satisfactory Essays -
Questionnaires are quick and easy ways of finding out information from people about a range of things. Written or self-completed questionnaires are the most common form of social survey. They can be distributed to people via post, email, the workplace and schools or completed on the spot. Questionnaires tend to contain mostly closed-ended questions, meaning that questions will have a few answers which the subject will select their answer from.…
- 473 Words
- 2 Pages
Good Essays -
As this method is quite time consuming for both interview and analysis. The samples have to be smaller than those for questionnaire studies for example the Twenty Statements Test discussed above. The quality of interview can also be dependent on interviewers skill and experience so two different interviewers could produce significantly different data with the same…
- 1030 Words
- 5 Pages
Good Essays -
On the positive side, case studies obtain useful information about individuals and small groups. On the negative side, they tend to apply only to individuals with similar characteristics rather than to the general population. The high likelihood of the investigator's biases affecting subjects' responses limits the generalizability of this method. Survey research Survey research involves interviewing or administering questionnaires, or written surveys, to large numbers of people. The investigator analyzes the data obtained from surveys to learn about similarities, differences, and trends.…
- 1073 Words
- 5 Pages
Good Essays -
A questionnaire is the main means of collecting quantitative primary data. A questionnaire enables quantitative data to be collected in a standardized way so that the data are internally consistent and coherent for analysis. Imagine how difficult it would be to analyze the data of a national survey conducted by 40 different interviewers if the questions had not been asked in a standard way, that is, if the interviewers had asked different questions using different…
- 6144 Words
- 25 Pages
Powerful Essays -
From the list, 100 will be picked through random sampling. The various populations and institutions in these will then be the target sources of respondents of the researcher. Various populations and institutions will be contacted to obtain a verbal consent to administer the questionnaire. A letter of consent will also be sent to them along with a sample copy of the questionnaire that will be used, as well as the protocol of the researcher.…
- 1152 Words
- 4 Pages
Good Essays -
Questionnaires was used to gather information so that the individuals would get more time to answer to the question. This is cheaper than face-to-face interview it is also easy to analyse the data and a quick way to collect information.…
- 864 Words
- 4 Pages
Powerful Essays -
The questionnaire is prepared in a well- structure and non-disguised form so that it is easily understandable and answerable by everyone. The type of questions include in the questionnaire are 19 multiple choice questions…
- 1812 Words
- 8 Pages
Powerful Essays -
Surveys and questionnaires are common ways of collecting qualitative data, and can also be used…
- 2627 Words
- 11 Pages
Better Essays -
The questionnaire has been prepared consisting of twelve questions. It has been prepared in a way to fulfill the said objective.…
- 341 Words
- 2 Pages
Satisfactory Essays